ࡱ> mojkl Rabjbjcc +vvXeeeeeyyyy8|-y~KT4   AAA!K#K#K#K#K#K#KMtP*#KeAAAAE#K?ee  48K???Ie e !K?A!K??rITeeIp0јya5 iIJ|NK0~KwIzQ?QI?IeJAAA +:   American Psychologist, In Press Terrorism: A (Self) Love Story Re-directing the Significance Quest Can End Violence Arie W. Kruglanski, Jocelyn J. Blanger, Michele Gelfand START Center, University of Maryland Rohan Gunaratna Nanyang Technological University, Singapore Malkanthi Hettiarachchi Foundation of Goodness, Sri Lanka Fernando Reinares Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Madrid Edward Orehek University of Groningen Jo Sasota Ohio State University Keren Sharvit Haifa University Abstract Jean-Jacques Rousseaus concepts of self-love (amour propre) and love of self (amour de soi mme) are applied to the psychology of terrorism. Self-love is concern with ones image in the eyes of respected others, members of ones group. It denotes ones feeling of personal significance; the sense that ones life has meaning in accordance with the values of ones society. Love of self, in contrast, is individualistic concern with self-preservation, comfort, safety, and the survival of self and loved ones. We suggest that self-love defines a motivational force that when awakened arouses the goal of significance-quest. When a group perceives itself in conflict with dangerous detractors, its ideology may prescribe violence and terrorism against the enemy as means of significance gain that gratifies self-love concerns. This may involve sacrificing ones self-preservation goals, encapsulated in Rousseaus love of self concept. The foregoing notions afford the integration of diverse quantitative and qualitative findings on individuals road to terrorism and back. Understanding the significance quest and the conditions of its constructive fulfillment may be crucial to reversing the current tide of global terrorism. Terrorism: A (Self) Love Story Re-directing the Significance Quest Can End Violence The topic of terrorism might seem ill-suited for a collection of papers devoted to psychologys positive contributions to conflict resolution. Terrorism, everyone knows, is the incarnation of the bad, the vile and the ugly; an epitome of the evil that men do. What is this sordid topic doing in a collection of essays devoted to the enlightened, humane and hopeful ways of dealing with human disputes? In this article, we argue that the same motivation that when properly directed may uplift humans to their most constructive conciliations may, when misguided, plunge people into mutual destruction, savagery, and mayhem. Consistent with the general tenor of this special issue of the American Psychologist then, we propose that understanding the motivational force potentiating terrorism may show a way for rechanneling it in a positive direction, paving the way to peaceful conflict resolution, and harmony in intergroup relations (see Bar-Tal, Oren, & Nets-Zehngut, 2011, this issue; Staub, 2011, this issue). This force is what we have called the quest for personal significance (Kruglanski, Chen, Dechesne, Fishman & Orehek, 2009), and what Jean-Jacques Rousseau labeled as self-love, amour propre in French. Rousseaus amour propre denotes self-love that depends on the opinions of others. It is a passionate need to count to be someone to be recognized, to matter (Neuhouser, 2008, p. 31). Though highly consequential in human affairs, the quest for significance isnt all there is. Rousseau insightfully juxtaposed self-love (amour propre) with love of self (amour de soi-mme). Though sounding near identical, the two concepts profoundly differ. Self-love, is about counting and mattering by standards of the normative social reality to which one subscribes, leading the good life in accordance with ones groups values. In contrast, love of self is about self-preservation, security, survival, comfort, and pleasure. In short, taking care of number one, and gratifying ones individualistic needs and desires (see Table 1). At times, the quest for significance may override self-preservation motives, inspiring individuals to make personal sacrifices for collective causes. At other times, the self-preservation goals may prevail leading persons away from collective, significance-bestowing, pursuits. Often, too, these two goal-types may be in conflict so that an overriding commitment to one may require suppression of the other (Shah, Kruglanski & Friedman, 2002). We will develop this theme later on in this paper. Our main purpose, however, is to offer a theoretical analysis of two opponent psychological processes: Radicalization becoming a terrorist, and deradicalizationleaving terrorism behind. These are described subsequently, following a thumb-nail sketch of psychological research on modern terrorism. The present paper builds on our earlier work on terrorisms motivational underpinnings (e.g., Kruglanski & Fishman, 2006; Kruglanski et al., 2009; Kruglanski & Orehek, 2011; Kruglanski, Gelfand & Gunaratna, 2012) yet it goes beyond it in a number of important respects: Primarily, it juxtaposes the motivational forces behind radicalization to those behind deradicalization. It further clarifies the relations between ideology and violence, and elaborates on conditions where non-violent ideologies offer a road-map to personal significance. Finally, and not least in importance, it describes previously unreported empirical findings pertinent to the present theory. Psychological Research on Modern Terrorism Social scientists interest in modern terrorism dates back to the 1960s and 70s when a wave of bombings, hijackings, and kidnapping catapulted the subject to the top of the worlds concerns. This interest spiked following the tragedy of 9/11/2001, the Bali bombing of 12/10/2002, Madrid bombings or 3/11/2004, the London bombing of 7/7/2005, the Mumbai attack in 2008 and the innumerable suicide bombings in Iraq and Afghanistan over the last decade. It is generally recognized that the problem is not going away anytime soon, and that it constitutes a serious threat to world security and stability. The study of terrorism is proceeding apace around the world; numerous institutes and centers have sprung up devoted to research on the topic, and the number of conferences, symposia, and publications on terrorism and political violence exhibits an accelerated growth curve. What terrorism is not. It seems fair to say that, thus far, psychological research on terrorism has yielded clearer knowledge about what terrorism is not, than what it is. We know now that terrorism is not a kind of psychopathology. We know now that terrorists arent crazy, even though their activities are extreme by general standards. We know that a specific personality profile that characterizes a terrorist does not exist; terrorists come in all shapes and forms psychologically speaking. We also know that situational factors like poverty, political oppression, or poor education arent the root causes of terrorism, though both personality and situation can contribute to terrorism in some circumstances (Kruglanski & Fishman, 2006; Merari, 2010). What terrorism is. In contrast to the emerging agreement about what terrorism is not, there is less consensus about what terrorism is, and what causes it. Some authors suggests it is the personal states (e.g. of trauma, shock, and anger) that push individuals into terrorisms arms (e.g., Speckhard & Akhmedova, 2005), that ideology is epiphenomenal to terrorists behavior (e.g., Sageman, 2004; 2008), and that it serves as an after-the-fact rationalization of undertaken actions (Bjrgo & Horgan, 2009; McCauley & Moskalenko, 2011). Others emphasize the motivating role of ideology (Gunaratna, 2005), and the sacred values that it represents (Ginges, Atran, Medin, & Shikaki, 2007). Yet others put stock in terrorisms social networks (Bakker, 2006; Hegghamer, 2006; Noricks, 2009; Sageman, 2004; 2008), and highlight the interpersonal ties that bond terrorists together. Our own approach has been that all these factors matter, and that each plays an important, though not an exclusive, role in prompting terrorism. Needed at this point is a theory that integrates them and elucidates how they interlock and work in concert. In what follows, we sketch an outline of such theory and present some initial evidence for its postulates. The Quest for Significance (Amour Propre) Theory of Radicalization Our theory departs from a basic assumption that like most human behaviors, terrorists behavior too is goal driven. Simply put, terrorist behavior is the means through which the individual pursues some goal. Admittedly, this isnt much of a revelation. Goal directed behavior isnt unique to terrorists. It isnt even uniquely human. It is pretty much how most animals behave. What makes humans somewhat special is that our behavior is socially grounded. Humans are social beings. More importantly yet, humans are cognitively social. Ants, bees, wasps, etc., though highly social too, do not think much. Humans do. Our goals and means have meaning that is socially determined: It is anchored in cultural norms and values that our group upholds. The application of those general values to engender specific motivation unfolds dynamically via a social process in which persuasion and social influence play a crucial role. In summary, our quest for significance framework highlights three fundamental elements whose interaction determines terrorists behavior. These are (1) the goal that the terrorist is striving to attain, (2) the violent means whereby he or she seeks to attain it, and (3) the social process that binds the goal and means together. This conceptual framework is useful in allowing one to move forward by highlighting the critical questions about the psychology of terrorism, and by suggesting hypotheses as to possible answers. The Goal Issue: What Motivates Terrorists? The quintessential question that our framework poses concerns the goal issue; it addresses the all important matter of terrorists motivation. The literature on this topic has been extensive, to be sure, and it has produced a long list of possible motives, including honor, trauma, humiliation, heaven, devotion to leader, vengeance, group pressure, even feminism (Bloom, 2004; Gambetta, 2005; Stern, 2004). These are all true and valid in a sense, but at a deeper level they represent, we submit, special cases of a broader, unifying motivation, the quest for significance mentioned earlier. The quest for significance refers to a general motivational force beyond mere survival; it has been recognized by psychological theorists under various labels such as competence or effectance (in Whites 1959 classic; see also Eliott & Dweck, 2005), achievement, self-esteem, mastery, and control motivations (see also Fiske, 2004; Higgins, 2012). The crucial thing is that effectance, esteem, competence, achievement or control are defined socially, or culturally (Crocker & Wolfe, 2001; Sedikides, Gaertner & Toguchi, 2003). That is exactly what the significance quest represents: It is attainment of what the culture says is worth attaining, the kind of competence that the culture values, or control over outcomes that the culture deems worthy, and for which one is accorded the admiration of others who matter to oneself. Ones sense of personal significance affords one self-love in the eyes of others, members of ones reference group, just as Rousseau envisioned it. To summarize then, we view both the quest for significance (Rousseaus notion of self-love) and the quest for survival, comfort, and self-preservation (Rousseaus love of self) as universal human motives that manifest themselves differently in diverse socio-cultural contexts. As we will see, these motives can occasionally give rise to goal conflicts, whereas at other times they can be concomitantly gratified by multifinal pursuits (Kruglanski et al., 2002; Kruglanski, Kpetz, Blanger, Chun, Orehek, & Fishbach, in press). Awakening the Quest for Significance. As with any motivational force, the quest for significance needs to be specifically activated in order for it to affect behavior (for discussion see Fishbach & Ferguson, 2007; Morsella & Bargh, 2011; Moscowitz & Grant, 2009). Even the most zealous idealists do not seek significance all of the time; they too occasionally engage in self-preservation activities, attending to their physiological needs, their security, comfort and so on. In our theory, and in the specific reference to terrorism the quest for significance can be awakened in three general cases, those of (1) significance loss, (2) the threat of loss, and (3) opportunity for significance gain. We consider them in turn. Significance Loss. A loss of significance can arise for diverse reasons, such as failure in an important pursuit, or a severe humiliation; this applies to the Chechen widows who were rendered powerless, and hence were demeaned and humiliated by having their significant other wrested from them by the Russian forces (Speckhard & Akhmedova, 2005). It applies also to Muslim immigrants to Europe who feel considerable disrespect, and often a rabid Islamophobia on part of members of the host community (Sageman, 2004; Kruglanski, Crenshaw, Post, & Victoroff, 2008). Rousseau vividly describes the violent physical and involuntary effect of his own wounded amour propre, when anger and indignation take possession of my senses: flashing eyes, an inflamed face, trembling limbs, a throbbing heart and reasoning can do nothing about it (Neuhouser, 2008, p. 71). The propaganda tapes of Al Qaeda and affiliated organizations (whose contents we have been analyzing) often use group grievance, the suffering and humiliation of Muslims in Bosnia, Kosovo or Palestine to enrage all Muslims and make them feel humiliated. Consider a quote on this point from Abu Yahya al Libi, a major Al Qaeda propagandist1: Jihad in Algeria today is your hope with permission from Allah in redemption from the hell of the unjust ruling regimes whose prisons are congested with your youths and children if not with your women; [regime] which thrust its armies, police, and intelligence to oppress you, for which they opened the doors to punish you. ... So add your efforts to theirs, add your energies to theirs. And know that their victory is your victory... Their salvation is your salvation. As may be seen then, al Libi makes salient for his listeners their social identity as Muslims, which in turn renders the humiliation and disempowerment of other Muslims the listeners own humiliation and significance loss. This latter strategy is not unique to Islamist terrorists as individual and group grievances appear to be a potent catalyst for terrorists motivation in diverse social contexts (della Porta & Rucht, 1995; Moghaddam, 2005; McCauley & Moskalenko, 2011). As Zartman and Khan (2011) put it, they (i.e., the collective grievances) color personal perceptions of individual hurts and provide the setting for individual feelings of helplessness that lead to violence (p. 28). Unrelated Significance Loss. Of interest, significance loss may arise for reasons other than a grievance ascribed to a known enemy or culprit. University of Texas political scientist, Ami Pedahzur (2005) cites examples of Palestinian suicide bombers who were apparently pushed to their desperate activities by stigma, ostracism and loss of self-respect (i.e., severe significance loss) for reasons completely unrelated to the Israeli-Palestinian conflict: A woman who had suffered stigma because she was infertile, another one stigmatized because of a divorce, yet another one accused of an extramarital affair, a boy diagnosed with HIVeach ready to sacrifice all for a cause to erase their significance loss, even though their humiliation had little to do with the Israeli-Palestinian conflict as such. Threat of Significance Loss. The quest for significance can be aroused also when one faces a threat of significance loss should one fail to comply with the normative pressure to engage in terrorism. Ohnuki-Tierneys (2006) recently analyzed Japanese Kamikaze pilots letters and personal diaries. It turns out that many of them valued life and were reluctant to die; unlike the Islamic shahids, they expected little in the way of paradise and its ostensive pleasures as a reward for dying for their country. Rather, they seem to have been actually pressured into volunteering. Their sense of shame, had they refused the mission, as well as the honor and solidarity with fallen comrades is, apparently, what prevented them from evading their tragic assignment. Hayashi Ichizo, a tokkotai pilot (Kamikaze) who died on his mission on February 22th 1945, wrote in a letter to his mother two days before his final flight I find it so hard to leave you behind... (he wrote) I want to be held in your arms and sleep... [yet] All men born in Japan are destined to die fighting for the country. You have done a splendid job raising me to become a honorable man (Ohnuki-Tierney, 2006, p. 173). Significance Gain. Finally, significance loss, or threat of loss are not the sole circumstance where a significance quest would be awakened. Another major circumstance for arousing such a quest is opportunity for a significance gain. The opportunity for significance gain may often come about in the context of a significance loss. This potentially applies to the radicalization process that occurs in prisons. The incarceration experience is typically humiliating and significance reducing. It renders the individual highly vulnerable to radicalization arguments that promise a quick reversal of ones abject social standing and the conferral of a heros status that begets others worship. Ali Ammar (Ali La Pointe) the famed Algerian guerilla leader2 used to live a life of petty crime before he was radicalized in prison by members of the Front de Liberation Nationale (the FLN). Richard Reid, the infamous shoe bomber converted to Islam in a British prison whereas Jose Padilla, the so called dirty bomber, was converted in an American one. Christian Ganczarski responsible for the Djerba bombing off the coast of Tunisia, and Pierre Richard Robert implicated in terrorist attacks in Morocco were criminal recruits to jihadism with prison backgrounds. Other wretched circumstances, not just prison, may foster dreams of grandeur, and prompt the seizing of opportunity for significance gain. A prominent example of that is Velupillai Prabhakaran the revered leader of the Liberation Tamil Tigers of Eelam, with a cult status to his followers, who prior to his LTTE career had been an ex-smuggler and member of the low fishermen caste in the Tamil community. In their recent analysis of 19th century Russian Anarchists, Clark McCauley and Sophia Moskalenko (2011) discussed the risk and status mechanism of radicalization illustrated by the story of Alexander Barannikov, member of the terrorist Peoples Will (Narodnaya Volya) organization. According to those who knew, what attracted him to terrorism was the bravado, courage, and daring that when displayed in pursuit of the group goals promised considerable boost to his status and self-esteem in the eyes of his comrades considerably feeding Barannikovs self-love in Rousseaus sense. Ehud Sprinzak (2001), the late Israeli terrorism expert discussed in this vein what he called the megalomaniacal hyper-terrorists the likes of Ramzi Yousef (the man behind the 1993 World Trade Center bombing), Shoko Asahara (leader of Aum Shnrikyo and architect of the 1995 sarin gas attack in a Tokyo subway station), Timothy McVeigh (the 1995 Oklahoma City bomber), and Osama bin Laden3. According to Sprinzak these are self-anointed individuals with larger-than-life callings and with insatiable urge to use catastrophic attacks in order to write a new chapter in history... (Sprinzak, 2001, p. 73) Accordingly, Sprinzak proposed to include them in a Great Men theory of terrorism. But the goal of significance gain via terrorism and martyrdom can be less high- blown and exalted. It could be inculcated early in the socialization process, or bred in the bone (Post, 2006). Some years ago, the Egyptian daily Ruz al Yusuf (of August 18, 2006) published a report about the Hezbollah Shite youth movement Imam al-Mahdi Scouts. These children range in age from 8-16, number in tens of thousands, and are indoctrinated with the ideology of radical Iranian Islam. According to Ruz al Yusuf, the objective is to train high caliber Islamic generation of children who would be willing to sacrifice themselves for the sake of Allah (awlad istishhadiyyun). To be sure, the tactic of indoctrinating young children into martyrdom and heroism on behalf of their group isnt unique to Hezbollah. Hamas is operating summer camps in which approximatey 100,000 boys and girls participate each year. These summer camps include extremist Islamic indoctrination, paramilitary training, as well as social activities, all geared toward creating a large pool of future recruits to the ranks of Hamas militants. The Basque ETA too has been known to target young children for purposes of ideological indoctrination, and the creation of future cadres of ETA fighters (Reinares, 2011). Psychologically then, the inculcation in children of heroic themes represents a terrorist organizations attempt to create an opportunity for immense significance gain in the eyes of young children to be attained via martyrdom for their groups cause. Motivational Exclusivity In basic motivational research in social psychology, we often find that when ones commitment to a given goal is enhanced, alternative goals are inhibited and suppressed (Blanger, Lafrenire, Vallerand, & Kruglanski, 2012, Shah, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2002). In this manner, increased commitment to the significance quest goal may banish from mind goals in the self-preservation category. Thus, whereas Maslows (1943) theory suggests that satisfaction of the baser (physiological, safety) needs is precondition for activation of the higher needs (love, esteem, self-actualization), our theory suggests that the opposite may also occur, and that activation of the higher needs may lead to actual suppression of the lower needs. The following is a testimony of a former Black Tamil Tiger, member of the elite LTTE suicide squads whom one of us (MH) recently interviewed in Sri Lanka: Family and relationships are forgotten in that place. There was no place for love. ... That means a passion and loyalty to that group, to those in charge, to those who sacrificed their lives for the group. Then I came to a stage where I had no love for myself. I had no value for my life. I was ready to give myself fully, even to destroy myself, in order to destroy another person. In this vein too, research by Scott Atran, Jeremy Ginges and their colleagues suggests that individuals who have become radicalized to Jihad are unlikely to abandon extreme violence against the enemy in order to save a whole family or village from punitive destruction by the adversary. Ginges & Atran (2009) report the results of a representative survey with 1260 Palestinians from the West Bank and Gaza carried out in December 2005-January 2006. In the context of the survey, respondents were asked: What is the position of Islam in your opinion regarding the bomber who carries out the bombing attack (which some call martyrdom attacks while others call suicide attacks) killing himself with the aim of killing his enemies. Does Islam allow such action? 43% of those who responded yes to this question (83.5% of all respondents) also responded no to the following two items Would it be acceptable to forego/postpone martyrdom if there were a significantly high chance that the chosen martyrs family would be killed in retaliation? and What if the bombing attack led to the destruction of olive trees and the bombing of his home town and school and the death of the students. Would it be acceptable to forego/postpone attack in this case? These results suggest that for radicalized individuals the goal of following the dictates of Islam, and thereby gaining significance, is of the highest order, trumping other goals including protecting ones family, and safeguarding the lives of others in ones community. Of interest, redressing the injustice and removing the perceived loss of significance (to oneself and ones group) through highly risky behavior (such as involved in terrorism) is consistent with Kahneman & Tverskys (1979) prospect theory that postulates a proclivity toward risk seeking for the removal of losses. The Role of Ideology In and of itself, having a goal aroused is not enough for a behavior to take place. One also needs to find a means to that goal. Typically, such means is provided by a terrorism-justifying ideology. It is the ideology that tells an individual what to do in order to attain significance. Such ideology does not come out of a thin air; it is generally grounded in the shared reality of ones group. An ideology is a collective belief system to which an individual subscribes. When the group is under (real or imagined) threat from its enemies, the ideology defines defense of the group as the pre-eminent task (Zartman & Anstey, 2012), for which glory (in the form of hero or martyr status) is the supreme reward. We thus assume that ideology is relevant to radicalization because it identifies such radical activity as violence and terrorism as means to personal significance, and justifies it on moral and effectiveness grounds. We assume these functions to be common to any terrorism justifying ideology, whether it be an ethno-nationalist ideology, a socialist ideology, or a religious ideology. Ideological Structure. Typically, the bare bone elements of a terrorism justifying ideology are threefold: There is a grievance (injustice) perpetrated toward ones group (religious, national, ethnic, gender related, etc.) there is a culprit portrayed as responsible for the injustice, and there is a morally warranted and effective (hence, significance promoting) method of removing the dishonor created by the injustice, namely terrorism, for which the perpetrator is accorded reverence and appreciation from the group. The ideology need not be more complicated than that. Yet, upholding some such belief schema is essential because terrorists actions (as all human actions) have rhyme and reason in the actors eyes, even if others may disagree and consider those unacceptable, irrational and warped. Moral Warrants for Terrorism. A major challenge to terrorist ideologies are the injunctions against violence toward innocents, common to most cultures and religions. To cope with the challenge, terrorist ideologies strive hard to justify the mayhem. Typically, this has been accomplished in two ways: through semantics and through rhetoric. The semantic approach involves language that delegitimizes the targets of ones violence (Bandura, 1999; Bar-Tal, 1990), often by denying them human properties and portraying them as disgusting infrahuman creatures (e.g., cockroaches, rats, pigs, or apes) that do not merit the consideration extended to people (Castano & Giner-Sorolla, 2006; Haslam, 2006). The rhetorical approach involves setting premises that imply either the necessity of violence against a specific target or the allowability of violence under specific circumstances. The necessity of violence is premised on the notion that the enemys responsibility for harm (to ones group) is fixed rather than malleable and stems from the targets essential nature (Dweck & Ehlinger, 2006; Halperin, Russell, Dweck & Gross, 2011; Medin, 1989; Yzerbyt, Rocher & Schadron, 1997). Such presupposition portrays destruction of the enemy as an exclusive method of defense against the inevitable evil that he or she may contemplate. Consider the following quote from the anti Tutsi newspaper Kangura: a cockroach cannot give birth to a butterfly A cockroach gives birth to another cockroach... The history of Rwanda shows us clearly that a Tutsi stays always exactly the same, that he has never changed (cited in Bar-Tal & Hammack, in press, p. 31) The allowability premise pertains to the notion that at times of war killing enemy combatants is legitimate, and the distinction between combatants and civilians is unsustainable because civilians are potential combatants (they could be recruited or conscripted, thus becoming combatants in effect); furthermore, civilians bear the responsibility for their governments activities; in this sense they arent exactly neutral or innocent hence they constitute legitimate targets for attacks (Ganor, 2002). Both the semantic and the rhetoric justifications of terrorism aim at portraying it as a morally justifiable, noble, enterprise likely to confer considerable significance on its practitioners. Effectiveness Based Justifications. To warrant a sense of personal significance, and to elicit the admiration of others ones actions must have a fair chance of success. Abject failure only deepens the humiliation and augments the enemys felt superiority. Accordingly, terrorist propagandists have typically intoxicated their listeners with glamorous success narratives that spelled the effectiveness of violent struggle and of the inevitable demise of the groups adversary. A well-known narrative, offered by the Russian Anarchists of the late nineteenth Century and echoed by the leftist terrorists of the 1970s and 1980s, was that terrorism would reveal the states impotence and provoke it to excessive counter-measures contrary to its stated values, thereby unmasking its hypocrisy and paving the way to a revolution. A similar logic underlies Carlos Marighellas mini-manual for the urban guerilla, adopted as Gospel by urban militants in Europe and South America. A different rationale for the efficacy of terrorism, grounded in the presumed weakness and degeneracy of the West (Buruma & Margalit, 2004) was articulated by Sayyed Hassan Nasserallah, the leader of the Hizballah in his spider web theory about the mere appearance, but hardly the reality, of Western (Israeli) potency. A similar justification was offered by Osama bin Laden, who in a 2003 sermon stated: America is a great power possessed of tremendous military might and a wide-ranging economy, but all this is built on an unstable foundation which can be targeted, with special attention to its obvious weak spots. If America is hit in one hundredth of these weak spots, it will stumble, wither away and relinquish world leadership. (reported by Ignatius, 2005, p. A21) In summary then, a terrorism justifying ideology is critical in setting up a belief system that glorifies violence against the groups detractors and portrays it as an effective and worthy way of making a supreme contribution to ones community, that merits vast veneration in the eyes of others, hence a profound sense of ones social significance (Rousseaus self-love). The Road To Terrorism: From Self-love to Others Hate An important aspect of our theory is that it suggests a functional trajectory from significance quest arousal to terrorism. The goal comes first, that is, the quest for significance is awakened by some circumstance. In response, one initiates a search for means to that goal by turning to ones group hoping for acceptance and respect4, and embracing its shared norms and values. Because ones sense of significance is grounded in the shared reality of ones group, an immediate response to awakened significance quest is turning attention to the group and focusing on what the group norms demand in a given circumstance5. To put it differently, arousal of the significance quest may immediately evoke the need to belong (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) in that group membership affords one a sense of significance, or of meaning through dedication to an important cause (Zartman & Anstey, 2012, p. 8). Consequently, the individual is directed toward significance bestowing pursuits in accordance with the groups norms and values. The collectivistic shift occasioned by the significance goal has two immediate consequences: (1) the empowerment effect (hence gain in felt significance!) on seeing oneself as part of a larger, stronger entity, (2) the sacrifice effect, the normatively-based readiness to follow the group norms and/or act on its behalf no matter the price. Consequently, where the groups ideology is terrorism-justifying, it may prompt the support of violence/martyrdom on the groups behalf. To the contrary, where the groups ideology is tolerant and benevolent, it may foster conciliatory and pro-social behaviors. These relations are depicted in Figure 1. Empirical evidence: On the Failure-Collectivism Relation. Recently, bits and pieces of evidence have been emerging relevant to our theorys assertions. Consider the notion that loss of significance (e.g. due to personal failure) invites a collectivistic shift6, that is, greater attunement to ones group and its norms and values. In an internet survey of 12 Arab countries, Pakistan, and Indonesia, carried out by Marylands START center (National Center for the Study of Terrorism and the Response to Terrorism) we find that participants reporting lower life success, hence presumably suffering significance loss, tend more strongly to self-identify as members of collectivities (nation or religion) rather than as individuals (Kruglanski, Gelfand, & Gunaratna, in press). Evidence consistent with this proposition was obtained also in several experimental studies carried out at the University of Maryland (Kruglanski & Orehek, 2011). In one experiment, participants wrote an essay describing their personal failure, or their personal success experience. Subsequently, their national identification as Americans was assessed. As expected, participants in the failure condition reported significantly stronger identification with their nation than participants in the success condition. In another experiment, participants were given positive (success) or negative (failure) feedback concerning their performance on a task (of remote associations) and their interdependent self-construal was assessed via the Singelis (1994) scale. As predicted, participants in the negative feedback condition professed a more interdependent self-construal than participants in the positive feedback condition. In a subsequent study, participants were randomly assigned to either write about a time in the past when they succeeded on an important personal goal or a time when they failed at such goal. Participants then completed self-report measures of independent and interdependent self-construal (Singelis, 1994). Consistent with the results of the former study, participants in the failure condition scored significantly higher on the interdependence scale as well as scoring significantly lower on the independence scale than participants in the success condition. Yet another study investigated the possibility that after failure, participants would prefer to work in a group rather than alone. To test this prediction, participants first engaged in a video game on the computer. They were told that their performance on this task has been demonstrated to be a reliable predictor of their intelligence and future life success. The video game was rigged so that participants were randomly assigned to either succeed or fail at the task. Following this task, participants were told that they would engage in another task with the chance to win a reward (a chocolate bar). They were told that they had the option of working alone on this task or working in a group. Participants in the success condition were significantly less likely to elect to work in a group than participants in the failure condition. It seems, then, that failure not only shifts the individuals mindset from an independent way of thinking to an interdependent way of thinking, but also fuels efforts to engage in collective action7. Collectivism and Fear of Death: The Empowerment Effect. Concerning empowerment, there is mounting evidence that activating or making salient ones collective identity reduces ones fear of death that (according to terror management theorists (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Solomon, Simon, & Breus, 1994; Arndt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Simon, 1997), as well as philosophers like Ernest Becker (1962) and Jean-Jacques Rousseau (1762/1968) represents anxiety about non-existence arguably the ultimate form of insignificance! In one relevant study (Orehek, Sasota, Kruglanski, Ridgeway, & Dechesne, 2011, Study 1) participants were asked to circle either singular first-person pronouns (I, me, my) or collective pronouns (we, us, ours). In previous research (Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Oyserman & Lee, 2008) this manipulation effectively instilled in participants individualistic versus collectivistic orientations, respectively. We found that participants exposed to the collectivistic orientation scored lower on a scale of death anxiety (Templer, 1970). Another study (Orehek et al., 2011, Study 2) used a joystick methodology (Fishbach & Shah, 2006) to implicitly assess participants attitudes toward death as function of their exposure to individualistic versus collectivistic priming (via the pronoun technique described above). Previous research has established that the speed of pulling the joystick toward oneself is proportionate to ones approach tendency toward a given stimulus, whereas the speed of pushing the joystick away from oneself is indicative of individuals avoidance tendency. We found that under collectivistic (vs. individualistic) priming participants pulled the joystick faster toward themselves, and pushed it away more slowly in response to death-related words, indicating stronger approach, and or lesser avoidance of death, as predicted by the empowerment effect hypothesized in our theory. Identical results obtained where a different manipulation of collective versus individualistic identity was used, specifically requesting participants to think of what made them similar to their family and friends (known to induce an interdependent or collectivistic orientation) or about what made them different from their family and friends (known to induce an individualistic orientation, cf. Orehek et al, 2011, Study 4; Trafimow, Triandis, & Goto 1991, Experiment 1). In another conceptual replication (Orehek at al., 2011, Study 5) a yet different operationalization of self-construal was employed. Participants read a story about a Samarian warrior. In the independent condition, the warrior was interested in personal reward and prestige. In the interdependent condition, he was interested in loyalty to the group. In the no-prime condition, participants were not presented with reading materials of any sort. It was found that whereas participants in the independent and control conditions didnt significantly differthose in the interdependent (and in this sense collectively minded) condition exhibited a significantly lower death anxiety (Templer, 1970). The Sacrifice Effect. Having ones collective identity activated may not only result in a sense of empowerment and a reduced fear of death, but also in a greater readiness to undertake risks and sacrifices on behalf of ones group. In a study by Orehek and colleagues (2011, Study 5), participants primed with plural versus singular pronouns expressed a greater readiness to sacrifice their lives (throw themselves in front of a trolley to save others in a hypothetical scenario) for fellow group members, though not for strangers. Convergent evidence for these findings comes from the work of Swann and his colleagues (Swann, Gomez, Dovidio, Hart, & Jetten, 2010) who reported several studies in which individuals who were more (vs. less) fused with their group were more willing to sacrifice themselves (in a trolley scenario) for the sake of the group, to more strongly endorsed fighting for the group, donated more money for a group cause, and put more effort in performance on the group behalf. The readiness to fight and make sacrifices on the groups behalf assumes a support for such a fight to begin with. In an internet surveys conducted by Marylands START center in 12 Arab countries, Indonesia, and Pakistan and in representative face-to-face research in Egypt, Morocco, Indonesia, and Pakistan, we found that individuals who self-identified in a collectivistic manner (as members of their religion or their nation) rather than as individuals tended more strongly to support the killing of American civilians (Kruglanski, Gelfand & Gunaratna, 2012). From Loss of Significance to Martyrdom. If personal loss of significance invites a collectivistic shift, which in turn encourages individuals to fight and make sacrifices on the groups behalf it follows that suffering a loss of significance, feeling bad about oneself, should augment those tendencies (Zartman & Anstey, 2012). Several of our findings support that idea. In a recent survey we have conducted with detained former members of the Sri Lankan terrorist organization, the Liberation Tamil Tigers of Eelam (or LTTE) we found that (1) the degree to which they felt anger in the last few weeks, (2) the degree to which they felt shame in the last few weeks, (3) the frequency of their feeling insignificant were all significantly correlated with engaging in violent actions and supporting violent struggle against the Singhalese majority. These findings suggest that a loss of significance (Rousseaus amour propre) may prompt support for violence on ones groups behalf. A loss of significance can occur in various ways. One circumstance in which a considerable significance loss may occur, particularly pertinent to young unmarried men in a traditional culture, is when they entertain sinful thoughts on matters forbidden by their religion. If our theory is correct, arousal of such thoughts may ultimately encourage support for sacrifice and martyrdom for ones group, designed to restore their sense of significance. Recently, we carried out an experimental study that put this idea to experimental test (Blanger & Kruglanski, 2012). Specifically, we had religious participants exposed to sexual stimuli (scantily dressed women in a Victorias Secret ad) assumed to arouse forbidden thoughts and hence sexual guilt. We first assessed intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity via an appropriate scale (Allport & Ross, 1967). Participants then looked at sexual stimuli or neutral stimuli, and we measured their sexual guilt via the Revised Mosher Sexual guilt inventory (1998). We also assessed their support for martyrdom for an (undefined) social cause, including items such as Under the right circumstances, I would sacrifice my life for an important cause and I would be willing to renounce all my personal wealth for a highly important cause. Those exposed to sexual stimuli, and who were intrinsically religious evinced more pronounced sexual guilt. What is more intriguing, they evinced greater support for martyrdom as assessed by our scale. Finally, in the sexual stimuli condition the relation between intrinsic religiosity and support for martyrdom was mediated by sexual guilt (our proxy of significance loss), lending support to our theoretical prediction. Quest for Significance and Pro-Social Behaviors Findings above suggest that when ones quest for significance is aroused, the individual may support self-sacrifice, martyrdom, and violence in order to gain significance. Recall, however, that this is driven by a violence justifying ideology that the individual subscribes to. Clearly, not all ideologies are violence promoting. In fact, some ideologies are emphatically positive and pro-social. They affirm that to gain significance one needs to be kind, tolerant and empathic. So, it is not the quest for significance as such that drives violence. In alternative instances, the ideology may inspire individuals to perform benevolent and unselfish behaviors8. Intriguingly, Rousseau seems to have anticipated this as well. In his terms, the particular ways individuals seek to satisfy their amour propre vary widely depending on what opportunities for recognition their social institutions encourage and permit... (cited in Neuhouser, 2008, p. 158). Work carried out under the auspices of the Terror Management Theory suggests that when ones mortality is made salient (known as the MS manipulation), threatening fundamental insignificance, priming individuals with positive values increases the likelihood of pro-social behavior. In an early experiment, having participants affirm their belief in the value of tolerance eliminated the effect of MS on the derogation of dissimilar others (Greenberg, Simon, Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Chatel, 1992). More recently, Rothschild, Abdollahi, and Pyszczynski, (2009) found in a Christian American sample that although high level of fundamentalism was generally associated with greater support for military force, exposure to Jesus compassionate teachings (e.g., embodied in dictums such as "Love your neighbor as yourself") plus an MS manipulation led fundamentalists to drop their support for violence to a level equivalent to that of less fundamentalist Christians. Similarly, for Shiite Muslims in Iran, death reminders generally led to more aggressive anti-Western attitudes. However, priming them with compassionate verses from the Koran ("Do goodness to others because Allah loves those who do good") redirected the response to MS, such that it led to reduced hostility against the United States. In other words, where the quest for significance is awakened, whether a pro-social or anti-social behavior is enacted should depend on the ideology that identifies the means to significance. Producing a shift from a terrorism warranting ideology, to one that identifies alternative routes to significance thus seems essential to eliminating violence. This directly leads to the second part of our story addressing deradicalization. Deradicalization: Turning the Significance Quest Around The same factors that radicalize individuals and turn them into terrorists, may, in reverse, deradicalize them and prompt them to leave terrorism behind. Recall that according to our analysis, terrorism on behalf of a collectivistic cause is a means that according to an ideology bestows significance on its perpetrators, hence attaining their goal in that regard. Accordingly, it seems plausible to assume that some factors that affect deradicalization may belong in the means category, whereas others belong in the goals category. We describe them in turn (see also Figure 1). Deradicalization via Means Shift As concerns means, deradicalization may occur because (1) violence is re-evaluated as morally reprehensible, hence likely to bestow ignominy, rather than significance, or because (2) violence is perceived as an ineffective means to ones goal. Priming norms of tolerance and compassion by TMT researchers may reduce the support for violence precisely for those reasons. These effects occur in the real world, not just in experimental studies. Consider the following statement by a former member of the Basque ETA (Euskadi Ta Askatasuna)9, interviewed by Fernando Reinares (2011): During the first months after I was incarcerated, I spent all my time systematically reading up on the Gospels... I gradually began to realize I was hearing and responding to the actual words of Jesus of Nazareth. Thanks to His grace, I underwent a profound and sincere conversion [italics added]. [it] required my sincere repentance [italics added] for past behavior, especially activities relating to my prior militancy in ETA (p. 800). For this individual then, violence lost its appeal as means of significance gain because of its, newly realized, moral unacceptability. The argument that violent jihad is morally opprobrious, constitutes a mainstay of the several deradicalization programs aimed at detained terrorism suspects in Muslim nations, or countries with significant Muslim populations (Saudi Arabia, Yemen, Singapore, Indonesia, Iraq). These programs, typically directed by governmental agencies (with their efficacy assessment mostly kept under wraps) aim to persuade the detainees, members of extremist Islamic organizations (such as the Jemmah Islamiyah, Al Qaeda, Abu Sayaaf, and others) that violence against unarmed civilians is contrary to the teachings of Islam, and is explicitly prohibited by the Quran. This message is generally delivered by Islamic clerics who engage the detainees, over months and years, in a religious dialogue concerning the prohibition (vs. permission) of violence against civilians. Successful deradicalization occurred in Egypt, for example, where two major terrorist organizations, al Jemmah al Islamiyah (the Islamic Group, or IG) in 1997 (whose leaders published no less than 25 volumes of exhortations to their followers to abandon violence), and more recently, in 2007, the al Jihad (AJ), a terror organization whose one time leader was Ayman al Zawahiri (the current leader of Al Qaeda) (for discussion see Kruglanski, Gelfand, & Gunaratna, 2012). The former Emir (commander) of the Al Jihad group (AJ) of Egypt (1987-1993), Sayid Immam Al Sherif (Dr. Fadl) authored a volume titled Document for Guiding Jihad in Egypt and the World. Along with other Al Jihad leaders, Al Sherif then conducted a tour of prisons to convince the organizations members to denounce violence. Successful deradicalization also occurred in Algeria where the Islamic Salvation Army (Arme Islamique du Salut,(AIS) abandoned violence in the period between 1997-2000. According to Ashour (2008a) The AIS had a consolidated, charismatic leadership that was willing to deradicalize. That leadership was influential enough to disarm the 7,000 militants that made up the organization, without causing any splits, as well as influencing several hundreds militants from other smaller militias and factions. Additionally, the AIS was able to interact with other armed organizations, FIS factions (Front Islamique du Salut), moderate Islamist figures and political parties to support de-radicalization and reconciliation (Ashour, 2008b, p. 2). Thus, the moral argument that violence is unacceptable on religious grounds can delegitimize it as a means of significance gain. It is noteworthy that both in Egypt and in Algeria deradicalization occurred in a top-down fashion, flowing from leaders of the organization to the followers. Such deradicalization process may differ from the case where the counter-violence argumentation is initially aimed at the followers, the relatively minor foot soldiers, as in several major deradicalization programs (e.g., in Yemen, Saudi Arabia and Iraq). Possibly, the leaders are more initially entrenched in their pro-violence views than the followers hence more difficult to dissuade; however once disabused of their extremist notions their influence over the followers may be considerable and the overall deradicalization impact may be substantial and enduring. In contrast, the followers may be easier to turn around initially, yet their conversion may be shallower and less persistent raising the prospects of recidivism. These matters could be profitably probed in future research. Even if one did not reject violence on moral grounds, one might relinquish it if it seemed ineffective for advancing the groups goals, and bound for failure, hence unlikely to bestow glory or significance on anyone. Consider the following statements by a former ETA member, after the Spanish parliament in the Fall of 1979 ratified a Statute of Autonomy for the Basques (Euskadi) and allowed free elections to the Basque parliament: Some others will insist that the primary goal ever since we first decided to take up the armed struggle was total independence [as opposed to mere autonomy].Anyway, no matter how you look at it, independence is not something that was ever going to be achieved by a handful of kill-happy morons, and believe me, because I got to know them well, youre not going to get very far at all, not far at all, down that path (Reinares, 2011, p. 782). In summary, deradicalization may occur if one came to regard ones radical means as morally unacceptable, ineffective or both. Deradicalization via Goal Shift According to our means-ends theory, deradicalization can occur also if ones goals have changed. Specifically, one could (1) prevent or minimize conditions for arousing the quest for significance, (2) one could induce the sense that that the significance quest has been fulfilled already, and/or (3) one could activate alternative, self-preservation goals related to concerns of safety, comfort and survival, that is, individualistic, self-centered, goals that may override the goal of significance, dictated by concerns for social standing and respect from others. Can one ever prevent the arousal of the significance quest? Often one cannot, especially where the loss of significance stemmed from personal, idiosyncratic reasons. But occasionally one could evade its inflammation by steering clear of acts that humiliate the other. Greater attention to the so called collateral damage, the killing of uninvolved civilians, avoidance of insulting portrayals (the Danish cartoons come to mind), the kind of prisoner abuse that characterized the infamous Abu Ghraib incident, and that according to international opinions characterized the treatment of inmates in the Guantanamo facility (Kull, 2007) could help forestall a massive quest for significance restoration that may push many toward terrorism. Occasionally, deradicalization may happen when the significance quest appears to have been crowned with success; hence the goal of ones strivings has been fulfilled. For instance, according to Reinares (2011), quite a few members of the Basque terrorist organization ETA decided to leave the movement after the Autonomy for the Basques was granted. One former member interviewed by Reinares explained it as follows: we reached this point where we had the Autonomy Statute, elections are held, and you say to yourself: okayWe got what we wanted, so what sense is there in going on shooting people and planting bombs? (Reinares, 2011, p. 782). A different case of the gratification is where a person comes to feel that she or he has done enough for the cause already. Implicitly or explicitly, she/he may say to her/himself: The struggle may continue, but I have done my share. My significance goal has been accomplished. A former ETA member put it succinctly: Look, though, my way of thinking about the armed struggle hasnt changed in the least. But Id done my fair share, Id given three years of my life to them as a militant, always at the expense of my personal life (Reinares, 2011, p. 798). Typically, the sense that one has done ones share coincides with the emergence of alternative goals outside the realm of significance quest; typically, the latter may belong in the self-preservation, and comfort category: Love relations, marriage, family, taking care of oneself, happiness, comfort may play a role here: Here is another former ETA member on this point: You say to yourself shit, man I better get myself a life, because time is running out.its a matter of being that much older, and in my case, specifically of wanting to get married. ...You are going on 40 years old, youre going to get married next year and you say to yourself well, shit, man I mean at this stage of the game to go packing a piece.. that would be a bit because you just got to..shit well, weve all got to live a bit (Reinares, 2011, p. 796). None of this is unique to ETA, of course. The emergence of alternative goals (in the self-preservation domain) appears in interviews with members of the Italian Brigade Rosse, (Jamieson, 1990) or former members of the provisional IRA (Horgan, 2005). We (AK) recently interviewed a Muslim Fillipino from the island of Mindanao, whose nom de guerre was Al Hamdi. Al Hamdi, was high up in the Abu Sayaf organization, a Muslim terror group affiliated with AQ and the JI. He had left the organization and now he is assisting in attempts to deradicalize his incarcerated colleagues. That is what he had to say: It became awfully hard to be separated from my wife and the children, and always on the run I missed being there for them and taking care of them, watching them grow up I also missed my work as a teacher... my life was hectic and stressful... I had enough... (Al Hamdi, personal communication, February, 2010). Terrorist Rehabilitation: The U.S. Experience in Iraq Critics of the 2003 invasion of Iraq by the coalition of the willing often stress its negative impact on the global war on terror. For one, it shifted the focus away from the Afghanistan-Pakistan theater where brunt of the struggle against al Qaeda should have been directed. Second, by destabilizing Iraq it encouraged an influx of jihadists into the country, and inspired, among others, the founding in 2003 of Al Qaeda in Iraq by Abu Musab Al Zarqawi. Thirdly, the inhumane treatment of detainees in the Abu Ghraib by U.S. military personnel, the news of which reverberated throughout the Muslim world, is generally thought to have played right into the hands of Al Qaeda propagandists who used it to fuel virulent anti-American sentiment among Muslims that may have radicalized many10. Less widely appreciated are the considerable efforts of the U.S. military to rehabilitate tens of thousands of suspected terrorists in Iraq incarcerated in various detention facilities (in Camp Bucca in Umm Qasr in Southern Iraq, as well as Camps Taji, Remembrance II, and Cropper in the center of the country). In the Spring of 2007, all detainees in Iraq, regardless of the circumstances of their detention, were treated as enemy combatants. Yet in many cases, the arrests were arbitrary and based on questionable intelligence. The glaring unfairness of this situation created a considerable pool of resentment, breeding a sense of disenfranchisement and humiliation that was likely to be exploited by the few detained extremists. Task Force 134 (TF-134), drawn from the coalitions military, was charged with detainee command and control, including the rehabilitation program. It was initially lead by Major General Douglas Stone and was coordinated through General Petraeus (for detailed discussion see Angel & Gunaratna, 2012). It included around 100 military attorneys and paralegals, mostly from the Navy and Air Force. TF-134 was charged with the determination whether the detainee is an Imperative Security Risk, or an Enduring Security Risk. Detainees in the former category were encouraged to participate in an extensive de-radicalization program, whereas those in the latter category were assumed to be too extreme to be de-radicalized, and in addition, posing the risk of radicalizing others. Was the approach to detainee rehabilitation taken by TF-134 psychologically sound? From the present theoretical perspective, the answer seems to be a yes. Several of its elements corresponded to ingredients of successful deradicalization as described earlier. Consider the policy of separating the extremist detainees from the general population of incarcerated individuals. Because of the arrestees anger and humiliation they should be particularly vulnerable to persuasion by ideologies that promise a significance-restoring revenge. In this sense, the separation policy reflects an appreciation of the detainees psychological state and the process of social influence that may affect them. Appreciation of the social process and the anchorage of detainees attitudes in the social networks in which they are embedded is reflected in the policy of involving the detainees families in the rehabilitation process11. A major element of family involvement was the visitation policy implemented by the TF-134 early in 2004. Families were able to conduct weakly visitations in which they were permitted brief physical contact with their incarcerated relatives (primarily hugs and kisses with children and wives), and in which they could share with their family members whatever money they had earned though paid works in the detention facility. Importantly, too, during the more extended (60 minutes in length) communication period the detainees and their adults relatives (e.g. wives) were afforded time for grown up discussion while their children spent time at a special playground in which they played with unarmed American soldiers. As Angell and Gunaratna (2012, p. 197) describe it: It was incredible to witness the interaction and see how excited the children were to play with the American soldiers. At the conclusion of the 60-minute... part of the visitation, every visiting child was offered something to take with him/her. There was always an abundant supply of stuffed animals in addition to clothing, school supplies and toys from which to choose Alternative Routes to Significance. Creating favorable (or lessening disfavorable) attitudes toward the US on part of detainees families represents good social psychology likely to contribute to inmates readiness to deradicalize. A more direct way, however, involves provision of alternative routes to personal significance (self-love) that do not call for sacrifices to ones comfort and survival (love of self) goals, as well as persuasion of the detainees that the way of violence proffers shame and not significance. The TF-134 projects incorporated several programs designed to accomplish these objectives. Attendance at these programs was voluntary; they included educational courses in Arabic, math, and computer training (all taught by civilian contractors who had background in education) as well as art activities. Religious dialogues with moderate imams, and civic courses were also offered. How successful were these programs, one wonders? That depends how one defines deradicalization. Between 2004-2008 the cumulative number of released detainees was 49, 632 and the cumulative number of re-internments was 3,145 that is, 6.34%. There are also qualitative data to suggest that in some cases, at least, programs instituted by TF-134 had the intended effect. Angell and Gunaratna (2012, p. 376) note the significant reduction in violence ad increase in intelligence that markedly took place after the initiation of the rehabilitation programs in 2007. There were cases where detainees have pleaded to stay longer in the detention center to complete the educational classes they were taking. Occasionally, family members implored the centers authorities to keep their loved ones in detention until their educational training was completed. And one detainee remarked: when I was detained, I was surprised to hear that there is a school where I can enroll myself. It was an honor to participate. It was run by the American Army who supplied us with textbooks and stationary. I attended an 8 week course where I learned reading and writing and Quran reading My thanks to the education program and my thanks to Allah (cited in Angell & Gunaratna, 2012, p. 205). These impressions are encouraging and probably contain a fair measure of the truth. They are no substitute, however, for careful empirical assessment of factors that contribute to successful deradicalization of terrorism suspects. We recently took a step in this direction in Sri Lanka as described below. Deradicalizing the Tamil Tigers As the US experience in Iraq suggests, some alternative goals and means may emerge for detained individuals under the new circumstances of the detention context. Similarly, many of Reinares (2011) ETA interviewees changed their mind as a consequence of new relationships and ideas while in prison. Recently, we systematically tested this idea in a population of incarcerated members of the Tamil Tigers organization in Sri Lanka. For over thirty years, the Tamil Tigers organization (more specifically known as the Liberation Tamil Tigers of Eelam, or LTTE) has been one of the most vicious organizations in the entire history of terrorism. It still holds the world record in suicide attacks launched by a single organization, whose victims were major political figures (including Rajiv Ghandi the prime minister of India, Ramasinghe Premadasa, the president of Sri Lanka, ministers, generals, academicians, and journalists). In 2009, in a decisive push to defeat the LTTE thousands of the militants were killed and the remaining ones (twelve thousands or so) were detained in centers where various deradicalization efforts were implemented. Unlike the deradicalization programs in Muslim nations that highlighted the moral inadmissibility of violence against civilians, and utilized theological arguments to make the point, the Sri Lankan program focused on equipping the detainees with alternative means to a meaningful existence. Specifically, detainees were assigned to various vocational ed. programs (including carpentry, construction work, electronics, as well as cosmetics, and garment training for females). We gained access to several thousand of these detainees, and were able to administer to them a variety of attitude questionnaires and personality measures. Among these was a measure of positive attitude toward the camps personnel (e.g., The staff treat me with dignity and respect), positive attitude toward the rehabilitation program (e.g., The rehabilitation program has helped me), and support for armed struggle (e.g., Armed fight is a personal obligation of all Tamils today). These assessment instruments were administered on two separate occasions; once early on into the program and then 6-9 months afterward. We found that both positive change in detainees attitudes (from Time 1 to Time 2) (1) toward the camps personnel and (2) toward the rehabilitation program, predicted a decline in their support for violent struggle against the Sinhalese, a major component of their terrorism justifying belief system. Thus, it seems that the degree to which they came to like and appreciate the personnel and the program, affected their degree of deradicalization. One interpretation of these findings, consistent with our theory, is the following: Realization, through interaction with friendly camp personnel, that the Sinhalese arent the enemy who divested them of significance may have instilled the notion that striking against them isnt needed (hence isnt an effective means) for significance restoration. Furthermore, realization that the vocational training they received is a valuable tool whereby they can gain a respectful place in their society, and hence a sense of personal significance may have further reduced their support for violent struggle as means to that end. It is of interest that the shift to a non violent means in this case is multi-purpose, or multifinal (Kruglanski, Shah, Fishbach, Friedman, Chun & Sleeth-Keppler, 2002; Kruglanski et al., in press): By learning a useful vocation, and leaving violence behind, the erstwhile LTTE militants may have found a way to feel as productive, significant, members of society, as well as gratifying their concerns for love, security and survival, thus being able to satisfy their quest for significance (Rousseaus amour propre), without giving up on other needs, (in Rousseaus amour de soi mme category). Conclusions Undestanding radicalization and deradicalization processes from the significance quest perspective offers policy relevant guidance for the struggle against violent extremism. Our analysis implies the importance of convincing potential or actual recruits to terrorism (1) that violence is unlikely to confer significance, and (2) that alternative means are available that are better suited to that purpose, (3) that there exist legitimate concerns beyond significance that warrant attention. The first two implications pertain to Rousseaus self love notion. They specifically call for deconstruction of the terrorism justifying ideology and demonstrating its invalidity12. The third implication pertains to Rousseaus concept of the love of self. It calls for restoring or elevating individuals concerns about their security, comfort and life-enjoyment. Deconstructing a terrorism justifying ideology means counter-arguing any and all of its component elements. It might require adducing cogent evidence that no injustice has been (or currently isnt) perpetrated against ones group, that the presumptive culprit is innocent of the alleged harm, and most importantly, that violence isnt the proper method of redressing the injustice. Effective counterarguing of these propositions should undermine the belief that violence against the designated culprit is significance promoting. Cogent counterarguing does not mean mere propaganda, or psychological warfare divorced from realities on the ground; it generally requires action on the level of actual policy rather than mere talk, showing rather than merely telling. This likely necessitates political compromises, and concessions (e.g. equitable treatment of disadvantaged minorities, respect for their aspirations for autonomy or statehood). It is actual policies, in domains of foreign and domestic policy, immigration programs and educational campaigns, rather than vague promises that stand the chance of persuading embittered extremists to reconsider their militancy toward putative perpetrators of injustice. It is also true, however, that unexplained actions, even if benevolent and constructive, may be subject to hostile reframings and reinterpretations by ones opponents. Good deeds need to be seen as well as done. Communication, and proper framings are as indispensible to effective dismantling of terrorist ideologies as are actual conciliatory policies that they depict and interpret. The media have a significant role to play in this regard, but it is particularly important to enlist here the cooperation of members of the militants community that have credibility to its publics, and can effectively carry the message of conciliation. Reducing individuals commitment to radical activities as means of their significance quest allows the re-introduction of goals in the love of self category: concerns with ones family, career, material welfare, security, etc. Reactivation of these objectives may instigate the choice of non-violent activities as multifinal means of significance quest that also serve alternative life concerns. Vocational education may prepare erstwhile fighters for productive reintegration in society; yet it is also important to set the conditions for their successful reintegration. For in the same way that individuals may radicalize and deradicalize, they may also re-radicalize under the appropriate circumstances. In short, their re-entry into the community at large and their encounter with their families and friends need to be carefully monitored and studied13. Coda The quest for significance, Rousseaus amour propre, is a distinctly human passion. Without it, Rousseau often emphasized, people would hardly be distinguished from beasts. The quest for significance is what makes us human, but it does so both for better and for worse. Where combined with destructive ideologies, fanned by sweeping social forces, it can plunge us to the nadir of human possibilities exemplified by terrorism, yet when coupled with enlightened thought, it can lift us to the summit of our potential, creating great science, great art, and inspiring human relations. A major task of psychology is to lead the way toward the latter outcome. References Allport, G., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 432-443. Angell, A.& Gunaratna, R. (2012). Terrorist rehabilitation: The U.S. experience in Iraq. Boca Raton, FL. : Taylor and Francis Group. Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Simon, L. (1997). Suppression, accessibility of death-related thoughts, and cultural worldview defense: Exploring the psychodynamics of terror management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 518. Ashour, O. (2008a). De-radicalization of Jihad? The impact of Egyptian Islamist Revisionists on al-Qaeda. Perspectives on Terrorism, 2, 11-14. Ashour. O. (2008b). Islamist De-Radicalization in Algeria: Successes and Failures, The Middle East Institute Policy Brief, 21, 1-10. Bakker, E. (2006). Jihadi terrorists in Europe, their characteristics and the circumstances in which they joined the jihad: An exploratory study. The Hague: Clingendae Institute. Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 193-209. Bar-Tal, D.(1990). Causes and consequences of delegitimation: Models of conflict and ethnocentrism. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 65-81. Bar-Tal, D. & Hammack,P.L. (in press). Conflict, delegitimization, and violence. Baumeister, R. F.. & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. Becker, E. (1962). The birth and death of meaning: A perspective in psychiatry and anthropology. New York: Free Press. Blanger, J. J., Kruglanski, A. W. (2012). On sin and sacrifice: How intrinsic religiosity and sexual-guilt create support for martyrdom. Unpublished data.University of Maryland. Blanger, J. J., Lafrenire, M-A. K., Vallerand, R. J., Kruglanski, A. W. (in press). When passion makes the heart grow colder: The role of passion in alternative goal suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Bjrgo, T. & Horgan, J. (Eds.). (2009). Leaving terrorism behind. Individual and collective disengagement. Abingdon: Routledge. Bloom, M. M. (2004). Palestinian suicide bombing: Public support, market share and outbidding. Political Science Quarterly, 199, 6188. Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this "'We"? Levels of collective identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83 -93. Buruma, I. & Margalit, A. (2004). Occidentalism: The west in the eyes of its enemies. New York: Penguin. Castano, E. & Giner Sorolla , R (2006). Not quite human: Infrahuminization in response to collective responsibilitly for intergroup killing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 804-818. Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth. Psychological Review, 108, 593623. della Porta, D., Rucht, D. (1995). Left-libertarian movements in context: Comparing Italy and West Germany, 1965-1990. In Craig Jenkins J. and Klandermans B. (Eds.). The politics of Social Protest. Comparative Perspectives on States and Social movements (pp. 229-273), University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis. Dweck, C.S. & Ehrlinger, J. (2006). Implicit theories and conflict resolution. In Deutsch, M., Coleman, P.T., Marcus, E.C. (Eds.). The handbook of conflict resolution: Theory and Practice (2nd ed., pp. 317-330). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Elliot, A. J., & Dweck, C. S. (2005). Competence and motivation: Competence as the core of achievement motivation. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 312). New York: Guilford. Fishbach, A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2007). The goal construct in social psychology. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 490-515). New York: Guilford. Fishbach, A., & Shah, J. Y. (2006). Self-control in action: Implicit dispositions toward goals and away from temptations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 820832. Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social beings: Core Motives in social psychology. New York: Wiley. Ganor, B. (2002). Defining terrorism: Is one mans terrorist another mans freedom fighter? Police Practice and Research, 3, 287-304. Ginges, J. & Atran, S. (2009). What motivates participation in violent political action: selective incentives or parochial altruism? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167, 115-123. Ginges, J., Atran, S., Medin, D., & Shikaki, K. (2007). Sacred bounds on rational resolution of violent political conflict. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 7357-7360. Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Simon, L., & Breus, M. (1994). Role of consciousness and accessibility of death-related thoughts in mortality salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 627637. Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Chatel, D. (1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does mortality salience always intensify negative reactions to others who threaten ones worldview? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 21220. Gunaratna, R. (2005). Al Qaedas Ideology, Current Trends in Islamist Ideology. Washington DC: Hudson Institute, Inc. Halperin, E., Russell, A.G., Dweck, C.S., & Gross, J.J. (2011). Anger, hatred and the quest for peace: Anger can be constructive in the absence of hatred. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 55, 274-291. Hegghamer, T. (2006). Terrorist recruitment and radicalization in Saudi Arabia. Middle East Policy, 13, 39-60. Higgins, E.T. (in press). Beyond pleasure and pain: Motivations working together. New York: Oxford University Press. Horgan, J. (2005). The psychology of terrorism. London: Routledge. Ignatius, D. (2005). Winning a battle of wills. The Washington Post, 13 July, p. A 21. Jamieson, A. (1990). Entry, discipline, and exit in the Italian Red Brigades. Terrorism and Political Violence, 2, 1-20. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263-291. Kruglanski, A.W., Gelfand, M.J. & Gunaratna, R. (in press). Terrorism as Means to an End: How Political Violence Bestows Significan. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds). The Social Psychology of Meaning, Mortality and Choice. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association Press. Kruglanski, A. W. & Orehek, E. (2011). The role of quest for significance in motivating terrorism. In J. Forgas, A. Kruglanski, & K. Williams (Eds.), Social Conflict and Aggression. New York: Psychology Press. Kruglanski, A. W., Chen, X., Dechesne, M., Fishman, S., & Orehek, E. (2009). Fully Committed: Suicide Bombers' Motivation and the Quest for Personal Significance. Political Psychology, 30, 331-557. Kruglanski, A.W., Crenshaw, M., Post, J. M., & Victoroff, J. (2008).What should this fight be called? Metaphors of counterterrorism and their implications. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 97133. Kruglanski, A. W., & Fishman, S. (2006). Terrorism between syndrome and tool. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 45-48. Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W. Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of Goal-Systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social Psychology, (Vol. 34, pp. 331-378). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Kull, S. (2007). Global Polling Data on Opinion of American Policies, Values and People (Report No. 110-4). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50, 370396. McCauley, C. R., Moskalenko, S. (2011). Friction: How Radicalization Happens to Them and Us. New York: Oxford University Press. Medin, D. L. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44 (12), 1469-1481. Moghaddam, A. (2005). Islamic Utopian Romanticism and the Foreign Policy Culture of Iran. Critique: Critical Middle Eastern Studies, 14, 265-292. Morsella, E., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Unconscious action tendencies: Sources of unintegrated action. In J. T. Cacioppo & J. Decety (Eds.), Handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 335- 347). New York: Oxford University Press. Mosher, D.L. (1998). Revised Mosher Guilt Inventory. En C.M. Davis, W.L. Yarber, R. Bauserman, G. Schreer y S.L. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of sexuality-related measures (pp. 290-293). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Moskowitz, G.B., & Grant, H. (2009). The psychology of goals. New York: Guilford Press. Neuhouser, F. (2008). Rousseaus Theodicy of Self-Love: Emil, Rationality, and the Drive for Recognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Noricks, D. (2009). The root causes of terrorism. In P.K. David & K. Cragin (Eds.) Social science for counterterrorism: Putting the pieces together. Santa Monica: CA. RAND Corporation. Ohnuki-Tierney, E. (2006). Kamikaze diaries: Reflections of Japanese student soldiers. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Orehek, E., Sasota, J. A., Kruglanski, A. W., Ridgeway, L., Dechesne, M. (2011). Interdependent self-construals mitigate the fear of death, augment the support for martyrdom, and increase altruistic suicide. Unpublished Manuscript. University of Groningen. Oyserman, D. & Lee, S. (2008) Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311342. Pedahzur, A. (2005). Suicide terrorism. Cambridge: Polity Press. Post, J. (2006). The mind of the terrorist: The psychology of terrorism from the IRA to Al Qaeda. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. Reinares, F. (2011). Exit from Terrorism: A Qualitative Empirical Study on Disengagement and Deradicalization among Members of ETA. Terrorism and Political Violence, 23, 780-803. Rothschild, Z. K., Abdollahi, A., & Pyszczynski, T. (2009). Does peace have a prayer? The effect of mortality salience, compassionate values, and religious fundamentalism on hostility toward out-groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 816827. Rousseau, J. J. (1968). The social contract, or principles of political right (H.J. Tozer, Trans.). London: Penguin Classics. (Original work published 1762) Sageman, M. (2004). Understanding terror networks. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Sageman, M. (2008). Leaderless Jihad: Terror Networks in the Twenty-First Century. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 6079. Shah J. Y., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 12611280. Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 580-591. Speckhard, A., & Akhmedova, K. (2005). Talking to terrorists. Journal of Psychohistory, 33, 125156. Sprinzak, E. (2001). The lone gunmen: The global war on terrorism faces a new brand of enemy. Foreign Policy, 1127, 7273. Stern, J. (2004). Terror in the Name of God, Ecco, New York. Swann,W. B., Gomez, A., Dovidio, J., Hart, S., & Jetten, J. (2010). Dying and killing for ones group: Identity fusion moderates responses to intergroup versions of the trolley problem. Psychological Science, 21, 11761183. Templer, D. I. (1970). The construction and validation of a death anxiety scale. Journal of General Psychology, 82, 165177. Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649-655. Yzerbyt, V., Rocher, S., & Schadron, G. (1997). Stereotypes as explanations: A subjective essentialist view of group perception. In R. Spears, P.J. Oakes,N. Ellemers & S.A. Haslam (Eds.). The social psychology of stereotyping and group life. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297-333. Zartman, W. I., & Anstey, M. (2012). The problem: Preventing identity conflicts and genocide. In I. W. Zartman, M. Anstey, & P. Meerts (Eds.) The slippery slope to genocide: Reducing identity conflicts and preventing mass murder, (pp. 3-34). New York: Oxford University Press. Zartman, I., W., Khan, M. (2011). Growing up in groups. In I. W. Zartman & G. O. Faure (Eds.) Engaging Extremists: Trade-offs, Timing, and Diplomacy (pp. 27-56). Washington, DC: United States Institute of Peace Press. Footnote Killed by a drone attack in Pakistan in June, 2012. Immortalized in Gilo Pontecorvos classic film The battle for Algier. 3. Anders Behring Breivik suspected perpetrator of the July 2011 Oslo massacre is a recent addition to this infamous list. 4. Of interest, the group in question might be mythical, imaginary or created expressly for purpose of the struggle; such possibly was the case with Anders Breiviks invocation of the Knights Templar as a group on whose behalf he committed the July 2011 Oslo massacre. 5. Such group could be a formal organization, or an informal network of like minded friends devoted to a common purpose (Sageman, 2004, 2008). 6. Our present evidence concerns primarily the consequences of a significance loss, though similar process should apply to other sources of significance quest, namely the threat of loss, an opportunity for significance gain. These implications could be profitably pursued in future research. 7. Alternatively, it might be argued that participants in the failure condition had their self-efficacy lowered and as a consequence preferred to join a group (rather than working individually) because they saw the group as helpful in their striving for goal attainment. In a sense, however, this interpretation echoes our notion of empowerment effect that group membership affords. Feeling low in efficacy (hence in significance) thus orients individuals to help-seeking which typically (though not invariably, perhaps) is social and dependent on others. 8. A somewhat curious example of significance quest directed toward (perceived) pro-social objectives is the Real Life Superhero movement (http:/ /www.worldsuperheroregistry.com/ world_superhero_registry_gallery.htm). Members of this group don superhero costumes and perform what they deem to be useful community services (neighborhood patrols, handing out supplies to the homeless, and in some instances actual crime fighting through vigilantism). 9. In English: Basque Homeland and Freedom. 10. Consistent with this possibility, in the aftermath of the Abu Ghraib scandal, assaults on the US troops (mortar attacks or employment of Improvised Explosive Devices (IEDs) escalated dramatically, while in detention centers riots, hunger strikes, disobedience and escape attempts increased in intensity and occurrence (Angel & Gunaratna, 2012, p. 54). 11. Cultivating good relations with the detainees families has been the staple of the Saudi deradicalization program (Al Hadlaq, 2011) that extended them financial and moral support. Similarly, in Singapore there exists an extensive and effectively organized Interagency Aftercare Group that integrates Malai/Muslims groups (Yayasan Mandaki, Association of Muslim Professionals, and the Taman Bacaan) whose aim is to provide assistance to families of detainees, in the form of financial aid, professional training for the wives and educational subsidies for the children (Angel & Gunaratna, 2012). 12. Assuming it indeed is invalid. Alternatively, it requires efforts to render it so, and to make sure that those efforts show. 13. Assuming it indeed is invalid. Alternatively, it requires efforts to render it so, and to make sure that those efforts show. Table 1 Properties of Self-Love versus Love of Self Self-LoveLove of Selfthe quest for significance the need to count, to be someone the sense that ones life has meaning in accordance with the values of ones society concerns with comfort, self-preservation, and security survival of self and loved ones.  Figure 1. The Processes of Radicalization and Deradicalization     PAGE  TERRORISM: A SELF-LOVE STORY  PAGE \* MERGEFORMAT 50 Running head: TERRORISM: A SELF-LOVE STORY  PAGE \* MERGEFORMAT 1  !vw# / C T H L  # |9X9cfghuw#8DFWsn !!!!8!;!F!U!!!f#y#####9&H&=(((hthz456hthz46hthz45 hthz4hz4hj`hz45 hz45Q !@uvw * L ^  dgdz4m$ $da$gdz4m$:m e#&=((,(.S.6_9< d`gdz4m$ $da$gdz4m$ dgdz4m$((() )K)O)**++,,,,--;-(.S.T.f.g._/////{000L1V1Z1d11111223(333445#5666666668888 9919B9_9r9;;<<5=D===>>BB CCEE1FGFHFhthz4H*hthz45 hthz4hthz456hthz46T<>?BEHzJeKQKSVX\\_a c dd,e0eeKhahd1$7$8$H$gdz4m$d1$7$8$H$`gdz4m$ d`gdz4m$ dgdz4m$HFLFHHzJJJJJJJJQKbKlMmMMMQ!QxQQQQcRjRlRsRyRRTTVV[[\\\L_Z__)d-djdmdJhKhahhh i&iiill"m+mmmmmmmn niojooo qqq(qrrshthz46_H hthz4_H hthz4H*hthz456hthz45 hthz4hthz46MahloruLuIvy?|n~YV$'Eb dgdz4m$ $da$gdz4m$d1$7$8$H$`gdz4 d`gdz4m$ssssCuJuMvYvxxy&yYȁ݁ނ߂ 01qV BTĊÏяm|:ĕ/0/9_ȪԪyɻhthz456PJnHtHhthz45PJnHtHhthz4PJnHtHhthz456hthz4H*hthz45 hthz4hthz46C/9dF% d`gdz4m$ $da$gdz4m$d1$7$8$H$`gdz4m$d1$7$8$H$gdz4m$ dgdz4m$ d`gdz4m$yz {RSQadGV`apReIz&'K[\v߷߭hthz4]_H hthz46]_H hthz4fHq hthz4H*_H hthz45_H hthz45hthz456_H hthz46_H hthz4_H hthz46 hthz4hthz4H*5v}~89pqr5A:;<=VXde(>@#deabBfɽhthz4\]hthz4H*hthz45hthz4mH sH hthz456_H hthz45_H hthz46hthz456 hthz4hthz4_H hthz46_H <9pT OW'C"-p d`gdz4dgdz4$d1$7$8$H$a$gdz4m$d1$7$8$H$`gdz4m$d1$7$8$H$gdz4m$B<r bnns!!! ! ! ! ! ! d`gdz4m$ dgdz4m$ $da$gdz4m$ d`gdz4fYa!A        / W X     gqYdDRSV^_boNZ$*jpikns!!!!r!hz4mH sH hthz4mH sH hthz4H*mH sH hthz456hthz46mH sH hthz4mH sH hthz45hthz46 hthz4A !!!!!!!!!!!!~!!3"""9#C###2$0d7$8$H$^`0gdz4m$d7$8$H$`gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4m$ $da$gdz4m$ dgdz4m$r!!!!!! #6#7#;#<#=#######&$)$*$0$2$6$7$@$A$G$H$M$N$Q$l$$Z%%% &&&&' ' '''+'x''''C((%)')ҿ笡硕hthz46mH sH hthz4mH sH hthz4mH sH hthz46hthz40J6hthz40Jhthz40J'6]hthz40J' hthz4hthz4]hthz46]62$Y$ %%&k&&'l''C('))))1***.++d7$8$H$^gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4 d7$8$H$gdz4m$0d7$8$H$^`0gdz4m$d7$8$H$`gdz4m$')O))))*#*$*%*(******++++8,L,M,Q,R,T,],,-[-U.W.-/S//"000000001B11ƿ~r~hthz46PJ]hthz4PJhthz4H*hthz4mHsHhthz4mH sH hthz46hthz4]hthz46] hthz4hthz4mHnHsHtH"hthz46]mHnHsHtHhthz46]nHtHhthz4nHtH,+T,],,-..E/t//E000S112W30d^`0gdz4m$0d1$7$8$H$^`0gdz4m$0d7$8$H$^`0gdz4m$d7$8$H$^gdz4m$d7$8$H$`gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4m$11111_22223E3F3J3K3L3424346474845=5>5?5A5a5v55]6{6}666677X8y8z8|8}888888888899t:u:::;hthz4mH sH hthz4]nHtHhthz46]nHtHhthz4nHtHhthz4]hthz46]hthz46 hthz4hthz4PJhthz46PJ9W334A44K55566p77 8c8888U90d^`0gdz4m$ d`gdz4m$ dgdz4m$d7$8$H$^gdz4m$d7$8$H$`gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4m$U9:u::C;;<<<=u==>i> dgdz4m$d@&`gdz4m$d7$8$H$`gdz4m$d7$8$H$^gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4m$ dgdz4m$ (^gdz4m$ (^gdz4m$ d^gdz4m$ ;;;;S<<<<< = ======>!>g>>>>>>>>?(?]?^?g?h??????ƴ𨚏q_#hthz40J.5CJaJmH sH hthz40J.5CJaJhthz45CJaJmHsHhthz4CJaJhthz456CJaJhthz45CJaJ"hthz456CJOJQJaJhthz45CJOJQJaJhthz46hthz4]hthz46] hthz4hthz40J$i>>?Z???9@w@@VAA"B{BB| d^gdz4m$ dgdz4m$d7$8$H$^gdz4m$0d[$\$^`0gdz4m$d[$\$`gdz4m$d[$\$gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4m$d`gdz4m$ ??8@9@?@h@j@l@m@v@w@A#A1A3AVAAAA!BGB^B`BBBZCCCD>DE&E(EBEEEEE~~uk_k~~hthz46PJ]hthz4PJ]hthz4PJhthz46hthz45\hthz40J&hthz40J&6hthz40J%6hthz40J%hthz46] hthz4hthz456CJaJhthz40J5CJaJhthz45CJaJhthz45CJaJmH sH %BCC!D>DDDBEEE)FFFG^GGHcHHI d^gdz4m$ dgdz4m$d7$8$H$`gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4m$0d^`0gdz4m$ d`gdz4m$EEEEF:FFFFF/GOGQGSG^G+HTHUHXHYHZHbHcHIII2IyII:JfJgJjJkJlJrJsJJKK&K'K*K+K,KKKKKK L"L$L&LLLLLMMMMMMN:N;N>N?N@NNhthz45mH sH hthz4]hthz46mH sH hthz4mH sH hthz46hthz46]hthz4PJ hthz4FIQIeIIIsJJK7KKK)L2LLLLKMM(NINNOd7$8$H$^gdz4m$d7$8$H$`gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4m$ d`gdz4m$ dgdz4m$NNNNNNOOXPmPoPqPrPsP Q)Q+QaQQ'R3RNU_U~WW_@_m`n`o`x````````````````````````綯 h|hz4 hz40Jjhz40JUjhz4Uh2;hz4_H  h2;hz4 h+8hz4hz4jhthz4Uhthz40J6hthz40Jhthz46 hthz4hthz4]hthz46]5OPgP|PPQQmRnRoRxRRRqS~T U1V & Fdgdz4m$ $da$gdz4m$ d`gdz4m$d7$8$H$^gdz4m$d7$8$H$`gdz4m$ d7$8$H$gdz4m$0d7$8$H$^`0gdz4m$1V]XZJZ[^^_ _ _ _ __@_J_W_$d$Ifa$gdz4m$ dgdz4m$ d`gdz4m$W_X_s___` `mmm[I d$Ifgdz4m$ d$Ifgdz4m$ & F 0d$If`0gdz4m$xkd$$Ifl0H$ t0644 la `B`c`d`e`f`g`M@@@ d`gdz4m$xkd$$Ifl0H$ t0644 la & F 0d$If`0gdz4m$ & F ^d$If^`^gdz4m$g`h`i`j`k`l`m`o```````````````` &`#$gdz4d1$7$8$H$gdz4m$ dgdz4m$ d`gdz4m$ dgdz4m$```aaOaPaQaRa dgdz4m$  !H$gdz4`aaaa3a4aKaLaMaNaQaRa h2;hz4jhz4UmHnHuhz4mHnHujhz4Uhz4 ) 01F:pz4/ =!"#$%}$$If!vh55#v:V l t065}$$If!vh55#v:V l t065±Dd 1Q6  3 Ab8d4Gq`aGBn d4Gq`aGPNG  IHDRT]~ sRGB pHYsodIDATx^} |eb)ٍ(d GFΖSBR@PDD j ?)Ai9$Ђ@Dyggggfwgfy'lwyy9jƍ)^D"@ @`t2Y"@ DH D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@H5:aGR:A⢮y*4RЬ29:WGσbD"@ruƗo< :sFmczVz {Pym.Tmsx/o+!D"PE,;3 $S%Gڟ+{9tI0jX;dD"P%G*KFSJwUB;ObcD"<HB^=1r^r&-Tij'MLMo j_^N4W)e({ŢUPe#3X: ;eOMsW7#߯ [-TQtwE`/:C= DT9 4w 6jB }\'Y DQQ-|-*˚ڄVԲ)uhj(.96YicW˨OjG^z0p9rG[tzBRG~Q3}k"@H$M6Z?ie'؆drOPk¨͑G4E9c ׭֔2~/ߌuuCm'}=YE Ъ-xcDU7o⨺q Md6u4Ūz!DLv5弈W,5ZSLs \PQ; /"D"@ Jߥ*(|>kyY$(L0lTQ,QTJm5,X=\#x]sMT,>NSN mcHAcSD"PBrP?U&.g#($U^, M,NVJrY7bݔ5WˑR.?Wnn,J绞{)@://[]LnXq[&D PMdAl0%X`"_3ϔu+`t;HiMvf E9"ՍDB ZGz,OTmp9n,F!8֨j&WŖDwYk#/vY/zw1GDFa!( D"@վI? D"(Hܜ, D"tH8"@ D Q%j9Y"@ D %}pD"@@ KrsD"@@ K DD!P[W_sD pꩧ.^P"@@C6$̙s$D`{ "@׿sAN[nݢEjkk#2$#Ql֘/_%IF6I^}ŊoFe@ DEzז3#D"@ ?n "@ D$-6J D?"@ D$z&h#;Օ+W'Ob-";Ho/.F? "@_wO@վ.8羼},z\عzK\,x5jIZ_j]4ªpϘ1cժUa ~;r?=}"mLw:Zf,zH-UMJB/=Q,1ﳢ^yQw /8ov].m++\W 4]|a@`f'D \ꂳ.^1go:,L$8vӳWO).[85"/WV.VKBcذ hI` VgϞZaZ~tq~ۉ0H D $"0簛)-:.mm,/^}?/ 9"0x30n[g;`foD#T' 3J0A0Ko&XO]].gq4rPerPHjjesuEFq?4e⿺`^7z-v4 5ؑjv%dM"@@!PW VKjtк-T >xP*zmo[Jݪ  00缒6b5Oz/rfYc) i(3JNUl ,/jd D % 9$YXB-3)rKa!X-"ٝY(p6t*`ҥ{gm{T(qMl-%bgZ4u{ep^3$DT1 %luq56(ɢ.r#N SWr}wо>xmˀS~ۃbgamף9/ Om4eπa[Iv܇N U@bɟbW {: Εa_oF^*@Xbeg4MW[T8CvD" T'4"%ֲyF!Q$pzW;-4I85/Bd'ʉͶEsz\|t4*>Lڍ"@Hzz7!roX/3OAŋUε'mm| hiS D#@-8pD"@*_XL/ o,FA"@5$^>"@  sK"@ D z8x"@ D8C^,M DX#@嫒#sU8 "@ QB/JԱNI&D"$a͞!8D"@C/0QQ珛"@@`5;"D"@@ D"@C/0 D"@G/5 D"$A͎"8D"@C/0KvmmoӜ1߶fa=1_k̾ D EbdR$\?3$Ls$D".$/{4#2s|Zho u}嗃"@H$y)ode5>ӿ}0%D"@*B"֧R}؟kiY qT;A֬SHӽ\`ڒeOWRVVPa  D" $\ឮ!P_4eJkfjBZ[[!ki7(Ƃu-JT]p%4lL.P*_Z[uG#R <70tz%bD"@_k(U#;Ru bMMcΗVZjS5rhX5N|#{֞$z+CTԸh&F3FeD"@@IHB 0NJNV<.WW(^&04AHl\JavZ?׍t!,]/_% D j s"Orζ5Cuw¿a!KmNʼn DD{AUp;_~ZEATi[D- tڲf&X*iv.M5~n{/٪U/3&Ge(QQ(tN& &NFsT7>eM"@@DO)<\9CxdUXA$phXՍcNs R;g.l:a'ĝߕLOqd!S99+{}qjiϭ}˺!`{Q"7^V\~ۈ //간sŤ6Qqxaw9Raqb y91B@[|7gTUOE2 D ڸq6QF/ȶzk.{ І L )mE:Pq_n@,0\7Jf !z }%qO{K<=@2cCM"@@t$45(设/*rD >wlā"@D 2?EYD+49(Juߊx'j,&:׼e"[.?bʛ[ >|Q?z^zh?zVk€" UrEG WmC3Cj@h*իgtaDh"'Qg0(ɢ]j6loP@'5!Ke ^]bq-0>u/g5*qF>>*en{.ڗ~W3Ֆ#ʕ .qC嫻"@@tȟxXK=ccw80U+ S[.WxO˒`uMM-r+aTDl ,>1{!ES%U<  D ZD }B VgI?nQJDbGk 8 2ցB[.m-?xDȫIauuùtΑ_{E[3%>+s7B=ODC"@|G 2޾?Kʼ u8ZsCo'HR+ Z_+&]fONFuUa}s="NCeo(`@5oA%2͙\"ՓU'wQ˭ C_%V6J@7n}L C2ey\!~Av D E`.fG8^E T.r{;"I0 ˮ|z#DH @eH "vIg_+R"@@QH9-.5L\)&opQ,_dD"@_V9 Veu͘1Nsk"@  UJZ+q+I]bE p DT!$]TbL D_@@ЍE1&g6I DinW Ak֕Bm /Er1 ^kUdZ/+۟z'!^5U]'DOD N.POf5m"o]S}Ջ2!/ #m"oGi+ L#[&kr_d]SXb7k2`MWy$+hU Dw3 +qeTAҫCvf,jz9*O%zwW\[L<[oo.2kU俖CW hi">YAQpcz7#6O@<P7R]N1;njF&$u/s]ʕW׸"YDHԠ@>`3 p"Nwonhwq¶D"P%"g}{K: (Ps\oRZT ohoI)]tVKi/khGJcJ5^P`R~`IcȒ?f#]0TߥKUUhGFjj,`lo)pɟ67`ggEpweTF3uWK 寞I_e;*Pfjۂɑ8at22tRt Hk] ȴ"X|w% sv2½i'ز^œY@1S ݝSO􅁛4ZþAT%.U  )Ð$! vCLaW'W6Z(-AXToc4L5^,rY%#H,w`~oؤ@4G%VI9׮lCzBW,IOE0^8M{1#U^Xttz_U.vlOMZˉkI5voĸ74<|RّmHTf}"@@CJ7)EmU%iҗ瑢툰AE +یvo&R kZ[RoaΜ D 0<%Bӫ2M0G"rKgYZA&Xú靌3ͭB (Xh9S^䭚3h%}M]0X" }e9oE/o=2V8'oN:Auk{҃A}?V-Y}XNêA_au{@C[Ay۽{jb+xZY}wHopcr7oiV;g;'&} wenxux{j<$USyN$װE< T!{ݥ/A5;H:lLӗr#&+'D xMd~N2P7hM}ֆÉ~iمxVN; ֭[ql.j\: @E/#F[¼V+V@v\m/3q'K}+S/z2 E_,&vJb*a$(z|K}_򗾾vI_jվݘ*S l!@_dV 1eL3HT'h\,&ZF/C!ͰRmIP" D @~Qg1Q"dh~&Av8 Dɟ_:kW@s؟,&PNK2lfXagD"@b_$ qS rO_Gα?YLdD,kՔǶ3eK.P༈ D Z g`id:E'uJd1zuy +%A_c뭷ߴ1U{>;s…r _Y x:|yCm< D F!jPwi'}*^)D)be7! "@ *0MI9Z!D4%D"@ Djj!D%Ո's8j"@ DBl5^7 DH! D" H؜* D"@H D" H؜* D"@,B"@@`z.:L@oO& 5  G` G'D 9H3m36mZOOZMMMNk<D"@țB+l"@0"{/=u.- D"@ K KX<%D"\z)򿰖l!Z`b!?&]]Rr裏8!k[l! A^dow9!4Il`b!?&]G7||Iw}wƏ__>@̼>ρ~N;e;b8"?P|ڂDOt kVO}j[~&5k~{1uԦ&pm6+KK^|?[k &nB?Mb``<<ӟt&a>Oy׭[kM8q֬YGq4b$Ö@Z][0pn[\ׯe]vi]wuM7`/"Xƍ<ȣ:㎋8ߎ;sK7Ho߀gwD;wnMM ӧO?;0" rgq'B"޾V[`!@.BO)fGD!6M?ꩧp p a(yҤI{o扶`8!@j 8#D8]{pr-ۻ> ߾pBL!\ f_D3ggsW;FV_W1;H1O9R@!y@' jvD!$dqH !ńI/l=X/  DG6OJ$2eJt86f*) !j/I]"@|D`ѢEuuu }zAfm>K/ >n`&o?fDnvb#WV#C 5 ?_&^6Ny/(=0󥛘4 A0pp18Y ?ɟزe"@/@9_?p66b-0{c8وO{'`, DOGj7 -'|UW 0)O"*$DxTCȁ}"sk6uT:Wy$C # ⢩øx!c/| T{iy >ɟx5"@yPTA)" "OoڦS GOՂtҞGNRg`)VaCC^|Tjv`1>Ԓ| 'NZ~ڙG5 ^HMCsiVMc.m_e!BH#\).|w׿;vhjXG΂X"m_sP"? Q"6|"pFe5 C]KzGj ZY)+ېVbMcz ݹ^3IJl$3a}A'?ymea'+  "@B?_n}vF[c<44 .h"j0*NKt*L00ܝ0);nwp&7XμtM>ٳgWFҋAʡ"(d/"ᬷνǰPǁz{qgPY__ܜMi:WOX'8?>-Za=(%/"Doԩ>L6s-il\˶ԍ TT2MJk`|I)Ңb]˜^'.<=iEKxQDWlUkkL,'܄~B_\YE2|^!75ۛnT=wuGz;[S##55[oA^6RtЯx*gY@7$ရr(_JxkDnzdX)"ϟ_ٺv5wL_3&$nQ>wmB~"M'A ?)S|ɞZ9EqWa>{>WӋ̔$4a̭"`X.cĉ9-%.f`Rn:gu-ӣy6(5C=ĜQZB? DGoG-~_J"r 1F!s=pg!@AK.cD+n&p be@wy'~FF'q#sX"x"'?I7Y'( u],XT'8Hm"nwߝPDv?y@^mfCwq 8a05kVZwC 8sVbvP#s=VeU?*… z)U0M!P/Or_rꩧ.^8K&bw}uQ.J,_CK{ȉ$X0^2?* 7a%EѣGgH]>#r5_gyg]fRD%m' w@SS>mLFf&:߯$E[;30:;y -^#SR+-̯iX2R4ujSf0akz{ ;.`1!J;׿uرm] wzq)ߚlssF6ɤ~tI8(zѝv}$Qkg/DzsgMK}T;ӟimoS[', '|!꺏W6Q_'^2;7ap9s!DO@ kjOoX "8zb 8nmgKa,H--Ƀ=Jaa!ZՊ3f̯붶x}4A$Q[jVz#d`Un ofm'^5+se54)Mg2)|.}u;Ë/t/$%+\?zBs[n*XTa=ωx, 66 Hmm-/N]$`.d~ae 'p:A+e!:fm82c'oúFMZu <442}ivm.a`րpd~0e!p:,Z6`גV"X_}ݽ#u-srNV0UҚhin)7r{!P>W;ʢ&tY$jr)ׯC:">Rkt/! DXA^zC$vR ":>J#ZHId~diWp-J?ώ?>q{{yU(@\&Pj_g7g 3(Y-܄nc="@cRKxX>z  MXxT_pf!m ;se-v\>W>;~]tQaE0FkI'|1Z2P/r8:i,"5wyV^L5 ]w5r"T.G =wuW|8q'/"8:߷~_uSSl;v„ (ƍ?E׬ דּr-=iȟ&&ìY?5v O=/jo'z{e<@dx 2po? nu'v?3؄Z vk׮{!'za BJ = {(ok">x<'x x`N?tJ"d)nFw8%jcZbf!Ѹ~ߗԤIE1^{%gAO# +ԧdxv\P [oo|`ؔha뭷T*Xb3e;/Ġ Wl67dж!g}$dٲeo?I>OGCn׿ z#S3ql %޳>%1c&a)чu.<ϥ _B{{{ dE `<)0;wdɘ"g B Q&؁IqN6 48WrAு()@ ž{i}i,7W;n?E{OTcU8&/Yąl]vA -ئnjW88e>Ʀ&VvX-CMϪUVAd؁\W^GGy$^EBV ㏃A8Qb7|<ua6KwKyu"xR*L Hy3!\>}iFY`d:tҟ? fPi? K3fPhX*gq-L8m@/RE.h$ad~`|Mdr <*eҽP 7n~f?k֬K.$ @_ iFV#W]uխ #8bZ |K ,[ 3I,(`DQF/jcãakN(U!rFi*,A\Q !W>Zv)O. А.)=:*q㬆 ;^VD"f #.L u1mn`%:o޼,pQٜ.y晸7^!<`!X>N/΃,`{>OoMs wlBf6O]#XSX}6Hu^e ;-mЬ[[\Li{47w?55ӟl2@P:sLh(^tqXxAEÆ 6h1\mj_о uWǎ8`$ȂL8rMJYD/~h"HpdD\s l ˄ 'C`xŐ+@ rN1/YӦi^+ y;9ŬWz oJςca{3%,Pf[[SJOerjSؙjX=ZcHo|=m-냿@0Xh{IlFٳgCwUCTB~]d2WZ2#tFSvZcMMc̼![d 3k]:WuZ -r:澎vůfEʠթfRw-uWg"F"f _^`qn5+! ` F)>s9WvYY E6^ N]@_gg'ׄ>^ /&apX2d}zq@#J k ~ou"IޒZ;RZh?Q,gCe5![ &5[8a|CQo`ɊeL "6j r1ؙ1&`w {l6<ldhHO [?,PmAH\;S be_W}E œ{qWV\%^߃`?<)? "Œւ ]} < anf菸=ŻR'@\.^ ;1%1H'{ j/6mV3Sl~f7pfq@jv ʂ`ww7nU`D;p4G4TAܐ:(PA=-'s ~!v⁗O PSvE կ~lxGJ?[MEd~ew  SeJ# #8&d~vv SD@ŻtS%e 탉A:f應3Ȥɞ]"IOlY m:_dts qK D:lii"gnyk',šKtcƧfX3 k .^x!šϰ~~{DCErmx~`~ tn\v.]a;'O|:&^{} >@>3U`##[?h/;:Va& g8ƦأT^uPQz2.u I)B|X7}l_M RQ!%Uio2Z [*`w駻R3mś8$''$dkln \[p'?t \#נ0ϠuQU~H_;]InFiH}/rH>h2Jr+apZ8*X1S槤H/IDRŒ jS Mܿq/Bj3p0$~Tu/Jf~k!āxoZ&cGbWa fݲhT2ӄrI*)/P xx+]>rض%)0eqq<'V0!?ȜŦ7|aoJzy%a[e1%EHV~p4d M$*t}kʐ7X~%u\^B`e-,YNa?p /%!vJJ# <)@`? 1}n\?7γ uj&-Ƃ0@X?0HlM\O O0z7?;{O}X.SHWn]yZu༟o޶(h׸CC40"|^QhgqY)GWx-`I9{tj9;YD3բ(~lW_}^@ Pܥ# 6l+mx|NuLJj730ez}zڕ+V/҂z*[g`)Pz)m_ %F){o^3R4aC`Sb,or,|jy/o#̝5i 2֏Z.[7'> N0SO-8&v]sszt,uA(y``Q]|G{T~q( 3iKKg_r:e BAL6=( 2uu5]2];LQ-H} 3Al3wy<<=({!4t- 3 X!O>紜tU+,vˆEx'Jې]!: =|~03B3Hc:jN6.^ څ_dkTG KgOnc^Q:#rhY?DSC2Jx$a|}w dŀ(*6<WP(*:3ljꎜ2 &e[0(uu9(!aCggg.r!cY)wݺuo34v ʥVʋݘEێ6nۅUz] A=NW8j DrNcĖXU1u.+j\jCXgC<,{Ž) k֋7$:Lbqߺ;1I*Bk <`'TB#ZRaMDIZ-*(/O2ͭebN oB짼k7KHq %..'VB^s]\u6|UWQ5ZR߂ޔR# KuElkʃPnieoRlLL|GtJUյ( DA?d袋є~欀J8Fc`*V'"QSOtM99լ-)O*|9:J^Iʆw< Jb:6r5}s=Ǐ } _fg1Ct+}w }zz9Pߒj(+8x펌 R)е\\y1݅2i;Dk"r2*{GxJ C3z+U#R:gv}zPрjoJNPBg8R Š'ITt>ץۮe- }.k'(+Ҿ~PQp5?am|![EVW^y%C6lSj> h 9WZuwWlL_~rP7Y a '/s-m`~`~ppPeE~6Ѹb "Oyf?aS+dp,MGA?[C-(df]p0o{8@4x[jCMKs VόTj_Ȗe@j[pqaNa 7td~Į0} O^33?jFyo|?Hގ-@ թa69k+ͧ>Ҋ+!`K1B(sgAܬF~ 0_vk1#Q4#'w̩_9>M7uW\\ lٲ;%"{ͯģܾi̓E椽:,++ RA#$o^5LHgq -P)q |_>UԾ^ <!:E ~4_C{M7E*Д Okl)PdohjQAڷdɒ(0? lZX^N˟w<~_GI}(u]@o8Q76ۇ%-_|Qݵ$ U6nh5b?dDO~ uEym6qFs]|݄?yE҅:?|)w}/PGA7m8Wx]4>uTnϽ[5 [ UB kCBJ[MXA on;0`u&B5Hh!eYp!> (e W]]G}#"/"C62eb^>_Bv&3dȓq#>> ;w.»l–]#d jF1cƸn$8w&ʎXZKyHO[Зe׊Zy p0O׸k@48eL`x }t2Ȏ3^qKd;/' zc熇/-46D{VTg3чggPB8=31*3 j ݲ։Q9 So4ɟ;;S--u l|/7!d<kNڋ'hI{?!T~ǥٌ}`AY Q~Ç<oFOiiQ@Tژv3"A%x]ԑO/ѽB|ܰnAFfGY~ BWw ! 9Sikvt>0k <oF a%&}oҥ_җ?(;؁sܸq@&=gdYjC<?Pih&Mm4iK#ߌLNJL>%N0;%J=6=aV$8!G-g8ǭTα G?fM g1&OX{kkNmj>737s6oN:$V[m6uwuium~bZ!4Ainm*ZLMMc<ۥH:QQu-1+ɀR+ơtF3S@2s -mA5 q4>O? IY)bAmdTvPk9Saoh!Ba&rVz֩_LXjqvlC*G*#Ў6ԋ eܸxwځ\ICJhTIZEm:H`"Yvm:ܗaA֤߇bgfJAU+v̖=QV_ˏʻ7+ 0kŪhP`z`ȗ]vY-v5H}:2KY&aU7JX9W+v̖=Q#rg߄+?0? 4ȬB\\sQ(ܬ>,ÍU[.ZF5 񹭢/tI_x n3mLq<ֵ\VQ-ja(xq.Z *̩UAKauu&݋)V&D{s+<&5 ¿l8l›իfΚ5k@ ;#/fF+dx/2˰ ka u"SF\qtʜcْ'jp%/ ܓAs[Cxt3);*{M VS2ƌɵlk4}1sK#&b˷)2,ņE&S욛jG>H#d {hu*~nV~6f0Nףnss] ϊn5Ոzb0anF]H`hGZGmveۏ{.0|bs@ 9ֺu * )?<~BZx>mP.x4ŐlkJJIilj^76_Grו[#d8&lX!=4_~IPD?Ǵ#Qy1[Dm=zZbr399+|PfzN"? eciuwPD*e]R c\_˃ u :|wy@χIpN"@3w>қ2L B٤[/T8R^ Hzv:e6 ;B_+HV9 \/"-;/n>0cVhR gD=Y6c^ wpj{yfc}1ϩ㨹zhs( 23?02?G`5׾!]ݘ@0߻\/d~S6R!>%1Xo.Żu/R1OB;AZộ:8lϦTk6:aO=#h P+ k`TP>v%P^qg xj>T=*Qw"U "Z6bד]d -68mp! 0LSRa#{6!Dѐos|'r`/`@7n܈q'Z{1‡gϔ y_}:_l"s wODk;w}u8 B An4"D!Z_T6">cAcߺRS!R\H1l2̯*- yJ[>"AvLف>:M7!WPlJ&>f K//Zh̘1z+1 hb-Oyѱ 8Jvډ7KaSbA?`# o @ȱ)Hj/E1 rW^ GG}Xe)`;$X &Skk9=Tz#) NbaO#D)"~l?2%s1 ;/bVUbbo,DZE'*6byuԱIc믧P@I A 4pId2QVSq!Jf'C/ʀ|)  \ M2WM  xfEH{qU,Re#a~ e,lH]VZ  vU6!Aw`ČAh`n.BD8 "0 8+|B> =P$K/a_م?~b`~.Rm@YI'{?>(d<t"9L&'ݖU)2;بW:.jZy_qޣxdNצ٪D2B<G*@yA%T_6߁& ӯ4dm/ hxhK?Fȁ,iiQp ayPn`{$G!T5㞥}rdh JsyU` h |[_‹'2amnAF)&I)ȱ1FqfB l#lhFi%*LH(8+?Lȏ5;}Yt1qD 5:h¿D~AWc{MՏ$M,,x/`1vB9ⳉhh!wCA*Iiw9%K(o#MS jU)O"G :|׏lkkՐăm0whw7t6sd⤽^(B/Ȋ Ƀje&M;/R/y&$[c0A /.ˡhUWjO^zQy rȜvi[n\"=ƵX?)Pv _[OHԔD!|cx@o?(|z;qD p za a@4`}Æ!ba0k׮O܈Ah)[*m9Oo(_G0SUAaah?gu.ՕR-)q2ل}]W?fsIwx衇38#bBPp0_|ZONYݜX+rYn.Q>=Cv;?W_Ty睟g#25i7ځ)F*9oNa8rڹ6WPOIuI93$._[8@YIt{ z^sNԶ%C^llMT%Q˜߇r^=tf\jcYxH*-=;$%LBXmϒaAwt$!\ܰa"Am H]pD2}1`lHfS+GۿI2٦H?fM'ljJpm缇VSLmZ;֯^m!YxkFg6fPc)mÈ5j(ظSWPpF`p?`HW[[U8V\H{PUJr(`89{/!9YyHtj}*EpA'26Szq0bceH!~#5$!HE_,;P >6{-$sV]AjAAK\ZBIeDT:#BTF&DR#0*CdH !AoGe&OHZxgmp +p9in6NiDhf* n7#ʔ`~-"[&,k^i'W-;P)7)=g%d3'wq3FI- ochtv~no}[SZ%l$fAg< P:/úqq\ujKǬZ1_@fJbzapRDsvhydwH1B g6.rh :@y|A gRkQ* 9| sߐG@;@\0.fމp2y0[9=ŊdV?;☲3Ds1VQ|Z Fuծ)S^w=ln6+فFZaڇ~ *.1KML}ROMCo؄Ta'@nn$։?![nR^{-tA劇{CAKdb޾S+0J3P[ ] a!Z 6۬o||p@}kVt ބ-ZOR3׀ ^B(c4!o'R,2OC[GIC%N(0A"5׿%_ ;08>қ֜m/UJҁ)%{gsqeǏ/믿.}5k/0<*\4, 8zΗaFX7a apz(2-)4 S)馕ԧ"Ɍ.KkNFO? {![Ftc . AS LhM?O#2)!Ɠdxvx$#|뭷x dmg? ~wcJ"'ፈ/qo-=aF\DIg͞^6R,D]/6;0"ل2ܣ!YUCcƀ8 Ƚ~W@O~N:N"כyJs8@A!_Pŝ^, ( kEBs'&ωuukݷ Z64VJ6!|+_A e$ڷpB(< *֐0pmA{ֆb[N"Fp?%uIaT k?S]}--W4.< N7I?-U$xFīfH([{8\c>d0F'11.P?,{z*3:(MTJ,[vIm>4y\8a!3ԛd[ۦt߼D)8LQ'}¦o)0͵65q{DkhFTTvlyvKM C{^lD0WZPa'xc]jW x Ҁ;>CiQ!͛>8FO3LX|ѐN!Pmct$0?ǁ10w-^"nժU9E~@>H?&4]?j8L6r²nڕU7f5ZV{J[gh|P]:5'N,pW\3TYs~+ovIuIX?H>$:1r Ȱ"K飅fCuS1+ *-k SpD}9) /hًҴe#"uMc+t Y&@?Y@#Vgg'x-#(Q\3D_7hf #ۇ` ocG՗ u-iP [D;p4geS8K/!d~b'/.[ZZdL^DJyDp`)9$8pM Sc# kRﰠm 흈bnym05&C_4*aGJhى}]K/3GC?imCmS8|}6'CCv_u'Nc⍋%)kYmPdYu& M6om%7J.ӄg'bv`} uR}Ågpe\$-|:m.u(ZM7!dذ3$vsL~=D;MA?/(=ܹ C w 4%fPm`UBbo"=ν8*9XӖ}MY6;Rע(}72 !)YZAM@ { {;c!1P_Y5;_os;,$曮̊"q |bOC :Οོzjp56?Ox^xN+ƽ=s5l h "!~_]wuFU/pGGǿotT7–O: k'ή%}đwgۯSs@ToIlеṂ1?* .}m$,`O?idG0D36Q &pe\=%r=܇A&.~m<CAgyg_/a}1+0\&ɖ!Gy1B/_/`Q bY"Al;XG}4_%CWZ3gΜ^JW%Pn%@OƷ|e~a# /u@ookxk*z!@F{ܹHo'?;HYtO^@So>*Znkk,ѷd~x3741s<뮻f|# a 5Dy{l_LFu> 8/H0p\\5?&qvaAio`~)L{~„ 8",=W蠃:8歷ނ!ڎ+ki'nσ Ȗqa AE7)WHO5?Tk,^?zA( UZ~mק g/Y3aܖ nL>4ƛE#MH-{88;D(XRZ2?{x/}s1k{\&Q<ܗCMFg'}Z(8S%WDC(! 2gO$lM3w…8 ,<؜Cx`̇cw~TĂAB|sPk (T!a{7FA Ȭl@ѿg+ B<00>>4K*Po>O^01 0U 6nf~ 0Z5Y5 (PC!L@d~.B?cƌǼyƏ`~H zWN Cőekkk# 5kRz0'F<_Z#UyE 4 7n\HdM맲~hdꛚbd 偺FFm+!NZ7&Ji)s5 S ͖yL4d~rH/i4HqP>vct y^" xE]0?j2K,~JugzIP86pg+9lm-[\j)x-ֆ+E`=Kn=8e_Dl{waHpGY!ˇjգ:x{w._asFb?Ӌu-9inK Cɢg_^\<<[t[HeB_wH]*$L{2У;:C:yuUc 6_bmnn.T%*߄PEAJ.CI +t뗨? "젿"+>vpwq8hbЍr}ѷz+=sCjHwp=;o4Fdk'" ~RZ|ҽUߪ򺡪f!x[ wbkf+ !p)R1'>3 lwB#'z4f\r]{sq^@'X>KBPu@P4A# ҽw:b9w+/op;WvL,z2`#4?x.(Ad>uuV&u#-~9u&ŻGU@&J_q+lЖQ-;-߬Y|v _ Giw-js꾖f+#O!Cbl8zҊ!w!:mh!<uz@+۩Ji߷N;m׋SGh&[\_sв-ᶤ'a=GOc Ym1ڗ0Qqߐ~+_׷ r3f| _]T1TPxװHQc7d3$BkgmDCtdo;R(; K/ؼ-]e]̆Bi,`VN<5qSG~'=ӧY ƍqk{[c˞cTbد#WGsG;@<餓z-P@dbUYr͚5xT2 xaFQǬ؜77$q^x!_"xǒI{oBH0 S֭C8%DpEp<`x A&Vkj 4*n @o`ՀOB ^KE * ^_Df ٳg% j:)1c` 4@fΜY.zl@fIV}Z64p@-EN@L9h>dBBVZv*/wwgӷWGGd`rL_Y6XnT80ӏ$5(wmB$%/xsrz gq<o?wHPȟ F7pC( h^{ s$mA8֑ /!2v5"fm3v?Zҁy`~"']w}PRF ǃ퍴]t$i@%YV_җXDa( d0*xuwyĬZ8Y v`6ˡPsSX8!BḵZU~ׁ„K_ΡN|vJ~x1']=b`*&DU!( d0?x:dB6|hkk# PP@8߀C+ !=d <  68Ph y@xÀϾ#r 7dձRB/0?s>hQQ.Q@ꫯ~G`~UKˢE^{GA D}0 Rr)mV#ذ|ofP__8Zy `:r. A Cz{<~ӟ ̡jP$ܮ.4Vo!DF$n{i># 18]uTj@ U܉ar&İΖ|CD.6Ga~ BȧFkH (9R؁06e@Y,Yb6\;=C]bqL_"ȟve: 3X>nW^-Lp | cs&l8#[r?h*)O8w bJu& ȐKs1QFD Rsn?۟h;ݣej%G C_v&00?&eu*Ĕ%i7 ўLa\!@'!0PsOr裏/P r!q6P )(%%WCHȈ{_ykHJ%.QFx[ԦU /K(Ӷ|] z$\nSEc{ ]œh`1ҲP!b\Pc{xBH[uL4 dqQp#cA64!uYwA+$9])Aj_ *_*V)EW,X "~eJȿ-ށQ1X3?Ý0:r;"D$@h=SvCF<d' [nod~UiUԈoo0.<@Ca~"A,:ݲ0VGbDT  ^xx@K$ZVZ:X5PTDpHif~ .$fT̏䯊-F@ |{!rp ƃ$ѰK/3/e]}͋FnG=1"P oAb=J⸬ϹrJt8f;T=# X""~! V M7t֬Y! wG6bD h#Rdx #?⸈6ǜGgs? D"E&*x˭YZ]nZ >P-}E$s`h#G"P-d2$3 g6k5w)݋,QpZ,G8 W,oe~#dwb~$An-E@r@,$֖TgݵԩꚿweNs&%?[R_G\svSuE-ɏrX8nhmF1?l<*G C{Ri5BFR5zaW# Apd+H&c Dk?i'f.^^*6y܆k_ev#+V&LAڙsgMojjʎƗ>V~?p[vrۉ(juy).E.J׫R-\\WHn ]9]oo}gkw6\[)WsM&cD$O!H u!8դs⭺%(sM7TnS:a( 4lLᓬkjΨ*c$-ÜTc=1Op܆9e~$l(J(D`FQd= [iEFs4qs~C>Xg'ZWlu[/] B =#{B%#BEe eSL ' If~$n4vE@@7|3dS~M dM[Z)Om sQwLT ϪH2AxU  g~$U9"@"2AïBW_7finV UQ{&Y`GhRVmmkW+SrMŊtQ% ~{&#~WE"@%A  Ei*f|¶Ndl?M[0taWg%K%{0O{fP.Zע j@P0=zt:aN&;jƍսҜ D ]oҤIAwM T >゘ͅa`l7sgғn+0S `&$>Tc@ٶ73΁9 W^=1! }}}q25_`"hn y`MC9Օ!(X\ǖ׋΃j+0)O{^rqDq1Rfaw@^ B%\dSVuV3ӟN>zg^!@g@?!DAg?^ 84Gly}$m)#3o2x6 #p-a|O<^{u5׸*Y.վC !/PgbV^} 'T  2bГ)1 ICC]fM&BaÆ?瘁_m@{"'xcOn[¯GWz Hߣ A >)qp^j\sd~e_YX"[l=Ez%rqd[Ռ dv8ZaDq2?;@g%!DxP)zjzMsU瞛;w_] -ɟMX" '.LΑ`M}j^LsP  Ihޏuuuo-}tIcŒDo?oڲJ&&Y!; iu{ u3lf%PYges,-<;  0gN2śʴs:`aC!Z_JÝ\\:yHօ 3*Ai~~8uIn'y&s6:,O ">\{;o^aS6N%C X0oҞSZZXzLvqcFzPZPiM ?RQѤ(eC-U݀vѕ<铔.0R;#ZWNh<*CGy?yM31G'!J*6 u"@*Eg}+*mE}B:R8 khh*0A^ޑR?$6TU}hp>ZG駟>w}wJ(nEo|of 9ɤDHAj{jXS GwUWٛ1K5 s +"@*E`ſo*ma}k%mCC]=`F`IV͒4 8stQ|\s 1*#@W $ǺD@C=g5Rin69vutյ SMT/[iT1R0!E/htʿo)b8K_ @o du"@@ESNc=>OWPʚ{=o\k:`+17> ֊ ?FFFT}.[ nKTKWN"C@5k ؉'&2 bC3mUw~3Ha=o C5#dSQ l"G?}URSH SEc 1wg&[$ R?d;D̘1㨣z'79ɚPR0 / xI'O`)"@@Eqp;8qbE=d6ig0/K9Uy~*Eɟx5"@{~D[z&Xp"Vȥ<_?!eD@dɒ#?q\.,^ oҥ??y82?oԷFl"@q]"hU dVd~B_Sd~aI|"@\"., 鲉WCH9hhăuY!`pf/D w;l%#l Ќ-@5;"DG[pagg+⾕$Մ,<̻ :IS\\6 f_D455=s_{{{? %&,?vi nH 8#DG_wPքf{g>SOѽ#L2ڸqcG"@}[m@DЧ~zժU-:#*uF^8}}};Ö[nꫯ~G;~wl_"@w D ~>q_MYP-3Ĵd~~[]JB"@An >lNP& ===mRͮ)P?Hǜ="@<<}.]6O^?wK i?.Iv9 D /^{GA D}0{饗{9N@U "@R";M7A aAOѣG_tE`rި3/>g8R"@@eK!ʃ>[o.;Sb \8n3f8ӡlҬM? "@@zGxhqmc Z!`tkk+4Qc-[&DDh6uta(XAǏ5nݺkŒ/Pl ~ D腑,>J \mm-jt/D4袖{Tr sH ʿ>䅥/:_8)߉?eD"@#5"@ DWH|"@ D ZEk=8"@ Dɟq"@ DD hGC D_ ^6N Dh!@h D"+$Ɖ D"-H  D"@|E{B7LIENDB`00000H08XV~_HmH nH sH tH @`@ NormalCJ_HaJmH sH tH ^`^ !l[ Heading 1dd@&[$\$5CJ0KH$\aJ0mHsHtH^`^ ,+tv Heading 3$<@&"5CJOJQJ\aJmHsHtHDA`D Default Paragraph FontRi@R  Table Normal4 l4a (k (No List @`@ -@"?0Header  ! mHsHtH.)@. @"? Page Number4 4 @"?Footer  !@B`"@ % Body Text*$1$ mHsHtHB 2B %0 Footnote Text mHsHtH@& A@ %0Footnote ReferenceH*@/Q@ @Body Text Char CJaJtHD/aD @0Footnote Text CharCJaJB' qB Op>Comment ReferenceCJaJ<< Op> Comment TextCJaJ:: Op>Comment Text CharLj L Op>Comment Subject5\mHsHtHF/F Op>Comment Subject Char5\P P Op> Balloon TextCJOJQJaJmHsHtHN/N Op>Balloon Text CharCJOJQJ^JaJ\ \}Dark List - Accent 3CJ_HaJmH sH tH .X`. .@Emphasis6]6U 6 $0 Hyperlink >*B*phF/F l[Heading 1 Char5CJ0KH$\aJ0(!( l[subtitle1 QilB^BB Q0 Normal (Web)$dd[$\$ OQ p'ft11 Oa p'ft12 Oq Pv>sehl^R`^ )Body Text Indent 2(hdx^h mHsHtHN/N (Body Text Indent 2 CharCJaJ^ ^ ULight List - Accent 3*CJ_HaJmH sH tH FV F  UFollowedHyperlink >*B*phR/R +tvHeading 3 Char5CJOJPJQJ\^JaJ6/6 tK0 Header CharCJaJ.O. Vx person_nameB/B apple-converted-spacePK!K[Content_Types].xmlj0Eжr(΢]yl#!MB;BQޏaLSWyҟ^@ Lz]__CdR{`L=r85v&mQ뉑8ICX=H"Z=&JCjwA`.Â?U~YkG/̷x3%o3t\&@w!H'"v0PK!֧6 _rels/.relsj0 }Q%v/C/}(h"O = C?hv=Ʌ%[xp{۵_Pѣ<1H0ORBdJE4b$q_6LR7`0̞O,En7Lib/SeеPK!kytheme/theme/themeManager.xml M @}w7c(EbˮCAǠҟ7՛K Y, e.|,H,lxɴIsQ}#Ր ֵ+!,^$j=GW)E+& 8PK!\theme/theme/theme1.xmlYOoE#F{o'NDuر i-q;N3' G$$DAč*iEP~wq4;{o?g^;N:$BR64Mvsi-@R4Œ mUb V*XX! cyg$w.Q "@oWL8*Bycjđ0蠦r,[LC9VbX*x_yuoBL͐u_. DKfN1엓:+ۥ~`jn[Zp֖zg,tV@bW/Oټl6Ws[R?S֒7 _כ[֪7 _w]ŌShN'^Bxk_[dC]zOլ\K=.:@MgdCf/o\ycB95B24S CEL|gO'sקo>W=n#p̰ZN|ӪV:8z1f؃k;ڇcp7#z8]Y / \{t\}}spķ=ʠoRVL3N(B<|ݥuK>P.EMLhɦM .co;əmr"*0#̡=6Kր0i1;$P0!YݩjbiXJB5IgAФ޲a6{P g֢)҉-Ìq8RmcWyXg/u]6Q_Ê5H Z2PU]Ǽ"GGFbCSOD%,p 6ޚwq̲R_gJSbj9)ed(w:/ak;6jAq11_xzG~F<:ɮ>O&kNa4dht\?J&l O٠NRpwhpse)tp)af] 27n}mk]\S,+a2g^Az )˙>E G鿰L7)'PK! ѐ'theme/theme/_rels/themeManager.xml.relsM 0wooӺ&݈Э5 6?$Q ,.aic21h:qm@RN;d`o7gK(M&$R(.1r'JЊT8V"AȻHu}|$b{P8g/]QAsم(#L[PK-!K[Content_Types].xmlPK-!֧6 1_rels/.relsPK-!kytheme/theme/themeManager.xmlPK-!\theme/theme/theme1.xmlPK-! ѐ' theme/theme/_rels/themeManager.xml.relsPK] RY TTT(HFsyvfr!')1;?EN`Ra<ah !2$+W3U9i>BIO1VW_ `g``Ra 6NQ!!!@  @H 0(  0(  A7 B S  ?XXw{))/LOPT l t + 1 O W U[HNQWjt Xaipx_ e Z)d)))))*%*******..O.U.W._.g.m.q.y.B/J/]/e/h/m/u/~/22223*3V3]3333344555588999999999999j;r;>>V?]?@@@@[BaB4E9EEEFFFFGGOGHHH)H_HdHXIbIIIIJ JJK)KQKYKKKKK&L-L9LYGYMY4Z:Z=ZBZ____bbcjjjmjzjjjkkll lll#l.l3lClHlPlWlYl_lbljlCmJm/n6nMnYn3p8p`tltHuNuQuYutuzuuuuub|l|||  $GR)/17QY17lt19;CGK &/ip8=΢Ԣ*2NU lwɯ ntu~egz|OP%CIxo{ ;?=F(IRPV9:BDwxACxy 1WXbc qsdfDQRV^bno  z { X [           Z ]     = > ^ _ y {               * +   RTpqDF01 68NOSU_`dfuvinrt!(ZeCI'2JZku& 5 C K T ^ i r '!-!4!:!!!!!"""".#5#<#A#B#I#g#x###]$^$c$x$%%%%z&&&&'$'t'|'E(M(S)X)))))********f+q+++[,f,y,,K-T-----..p/v///0000001%1t1~111112t2u222222 33)3/33375?5b5p5556g6j667]7^7g7h7777777778898>8?8h8j8l8n8v8w8888888888999999":+:{::;;L;R;;;><D<J<P<z<<<<B=J===q?z???@@AAeAlAAAABBBB'B7C?CCCCCCC2D:DDDDDE EE!EEEIFQFWF_FjFnFG GGG G(GGGGGGG|HHHHHIIIIIJJJJJJKK LLLLSS)T/TTTTTTU9U?UUUXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXYY3YNYPYSY .>H 5=ux&&''()::==AAAABBZFcF,]/]Zd\dg hj$jOmPmww+/9`GS.0~su$^by=Vz{dfBe>B  `qr abmo    mnrt Dq~Wr2XYk!YZ.2@i{}~*lx& 5  %!!!!"c"f"i""""B#K###$7$]$b$$$%#%$%%%%%&&&E'H'''''((((()_***++V+++,@,,,a--]......///0c0f000U11112t2u222;33333444R44445{556h6i6666667Y7Z777778898v888k999 :G:_:::;;!<=<<<<<(=@=`=d=====)>9>?.?/?]???@@+@b@{@@BB7CNCOCCCC2DFDDDDDKEPEEEEFIF}FFFFGG4GGGGGgH{H|HHJJLLOPRRXW[WsWvWWWWW XXBXJXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXYY3YNYPYSY::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::(= "h\G)ޠt ^`OJQJo( 8^8`OJQJo( ^`OJQJo(o  p^ `OJQJo(  @ ^ `OJQJo( x^x`OJQJo( H^H`OJQJo(o ^`OJQJo( ^`OJQJo( ^`OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  pp^p`OJQJo("  @ @ ^@ `OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  PP^P`OJQJo(" 8^8`o(. ^`hH.  L^ `LhH.  ^ `hH. x^x`hH. HL^H`LhH. ^`hH. ^`hH. L^`LhH. ^`OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  pp^p`OJQJo("  @ @ ^@ `OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  ^`OJQJo("  PP^P`OJQJo(" )(= \G:R:̭DTr>,^Jz"P        *nrä(z$ڃ* & 4QH@ &x)t(&=)t()D)t(G_)t(PwsH{x |1|z4W@WJWWWXW XcXdXXXXXXPYSY@RYP@Unknown GTimes New Roman5Symbol3 ArialG5  jMS Mincho-3 fg7CambriakTimesNewRomanPSMTTimes New Roman5& *[`)Tahoma?1M Courier New;Wingdings 1hѼ Ѽ  u192^r3A%2qAx4 ]X 2qP$P)h2 fOn the Psychology of Positive Transformation: Re-chaneling the Quest for Significance to End Terrorism A KruglanskiAnna Sheveland     Oh+'0,8L dp    'hOn the Psychology of Positive Transformation: Re-chaneling the Quest for Significance to End TerrorismA Kruglanski Normal.dotmAnna Sheveland2Microsoft Macintosh Word@@bB@y@y219 ՜.+,0d hp  C'Univerisity of Maryland^ ] gOn the Psychology of Positive Transformation: Re-chaneling the Quest for Significance to End Terrorism Title  !"#$%&'()*+,-./0123456789:;<=>?@ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSTUVWXYZ[\]^_`abcdefghijklmnopqrstuvwxyz{|}~      !"#$%&'()*+,-./023456789:;<=>?@ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSTUVWXY[\]^_`acdefghinRoot Entry F.֘pData 1Table1QWordDocument+SummaryInformation(ZDocumentSummaryInformation8bCompObj`ObjectPool5.֘5.֘ F Microsoft Word 97-2004 DocumentNB6WWord.Document.8