ࡱ> egd%` 0V<bjbj 4X̟̟V44470:jjjjEEEt7v7v7v7v7v7v7$8hO;7*EE**7jj7///*@jjt7/*t7///j$ pA߷./H7,707/-<.-</-</EW!/?$T&KEEE77/ EEE7****$  The Impact of Accent Stereotypes on Service Outcomes and Its Boundary Conditions Ze Wang School of Business, University of Kansas, 1300 Sunnyside Avenue, Summerfield 118I Lawrence, KS 66045 Phone: 785-864-1804 E-mail:  HYPERLINK "mailto:amywang@ku.edu" amywang@ku.edu Aaron Arndt College of Business& Public Administration, Old Dominion University Constant Hall 2147 Norfolk, VA 23529 Phone: 405-924-8175 E-mail:  HYPERLINK "mailto:aarndt@odu.edu" aarndt@odu.edu Surendra Singh School of Business, University of Kansas, 1300 Sunnyside Avenue, Summerfield 226L Lawrence, KS 66045 Phone: 785-864-7531 E-mail:  HYPERLINK "mailto:ssingh@ku.edu" ssingh@ku.edu Monica Biernat Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, 1415 Jayhawk Blvd., Fraser Hall 426 Lawrence, KS 66045 Phone: 785-864-9815 E-mail: biernat@ku.edu The Impact of Accent Stereotypes on Service Outcomes and Its Boundary Conditions EXTENDED ABSTRACT When we listened back to calls people had complained about often they were fine. Some people wanted the member of staff to fail because they were in India. I dont know why that should be, but when customers start voting with their feet, you have to respond. --Adrian Web (Call Center Manager, BBC news, 2/14/2007) The accent of service providers may positively or negatively bias customer perception of service qualityespeciallyin service contexts where visual cues are absent (e.g., call centers). In this research, we explore the effects of accent stereotypes in a variety of call center situations. With two laboratory experiments, we demonstrate that even with identical service outcomes, customers perception and interpretation of their service experience changed as a function of customer service employee accent (i.e., British, Indian and American). However, biases caused by accent stereotyping decrease when relevant objective information is available (i.e., the industrial norm). Sociolinguistics literature (Lippi-Green 1994, Giles and Powesland 1975) suggests that accent is an important indicator of ones ethnicity, regional affiliation and social class. Even though accents may be subtle, individuals are still able to perceive and distinguish among different accents (Cargile 2000; Giles, Williams, Mackie and Rosselli 1995). People attribute positive traits to certain types of accents based on the prestige of the class or group that possess it (e.g., sophistication and politeness associated with British accent) (Ladegaard 1998). In contrast, people also discriminate against the speakers with foreign accents (e.g., African-American, Indian, and Mexican-American) which may link to skin that isnt white, or signal a third-party homeland (Lippi-Green, 1997, p.239). Peoples general tendency to evaluate individuals performance more harshly than others upon accent has been a persistent finding in extant literature (Baugh 2000; Purkiss et al. 2000). We extend these theories by examining accent stereotyping effect in the service context and testing its influence on customer satisfaction and customer evaluation of employee performance. Additionally, based on previous literature on stereotypical influence on causal attributions (Hewstone 1990; Jackson, Sullivan and Hodge1993), individuals tend to attribute stereotype-consistent behavior to internal stable causes and stereotype-inconsistent behavior to external unstable causes. We hypothesize that American customers are more likely to attribute favorable service to individual employee factors and attribute unfavorable service to external firm factors when an employee has an American or British accent. However, the attributions are reversed for employees with an Indian accent. The impacts of accent stereotype on service outcomes depend on the informational context during the service. Research on stereotyping effect (Kunda and Thagard 1996) suggest that stereotyping is most likely to occur if customers do not have background information or other objective cues to make their judgment. Thus we hypothesize that the influence of employees accent on customers perception and evaluation of the service encounter should disappear, or at least decease, if customers are informed of industrial norms. We conducted two empirical studies to test our hypotheses. The first studywas designed to test the hypotheses that the accent stereotypes will bias customers perception and evaluation of their service experience. Participants were asked to listen to taped phone calls between a customer and a bank employee from the perspective of the customer. Consistent with our hypotheses, we found that accent stereotypes influence both customer evaluation of employee performance and customer satisfaction. Even though service scenarios and conversation scripts were identical, customers rated the performance of employees with Indian accents much lower than the performance of employees with American and British accents. The influence of accent stereotyping on customer satisfaction was moderated by the service outcome. When the service outcome was favorable, the British accent was rated more positively than the American accent but, contrary to our hypothesis, the Indian accent was not rated more negatively than the American accent. However, when the service outcome was unfavorable, customer satisfaction in the Indian-accent condition was significantly lower than it was in American or British accent condition. The second studyexamined the moderating effects of information availability. We tested whether accent stereotypical effectswould decrease with the presence of other judgmental cues. In the high information condition, customers were informed of the industrial norms about the specific type of service; whereas in the low information condition, customers were not provided with any background information. As predicted, accent stereotypical effects on customer satisfaction were no longer significant after customers had more information to make judgment. However, the negative bias against the Indian accent was so strong that even when customers were informed of industrial norms, they still rated Indian-employees performance as the lowest. Another noteworthy finding fromthis experiment was the difference in customer causal attribution across conditions. We measured customers attributions by coding open-ended question responses. When employees from favorable outgroup (British) or ingroup (American) performed well, customers attributed this favorable outcome to their internal factors. When they provided poor service, customers attributed this unfavorable outcome to the external factors. The reverse is true when the employee was from an unfavorable outgroup (Indian). In summary, we show across two studies that accent stereotypes bias customer evaluation and interpretation of service outcomes. Whereas they display a positive bias toward the British accent, customers show a strong negative bias against the Indian accent. They also make stereotype-consistent attributions about service outcomes. These stereotypical effects on service outcomes are partially reduced when customers are informed of other relevant information. REFERENCES Baugh, John (2000), Racial Identification by Speech, American Speech, 75(4), 362-364 Cargile, Aaron Castelan (2000), Evaluations of Employment Suitability: Does Accent Always Matter? Journal of Employment Counseling, 37 (3), 165-177. Cowen, David A. (1986), Developing a Process Model of Problem Recognition, Academy of Management Review, 11 (4): 763-776. DeShields Jr., Oscar W and Gilberto de los Santos (2000), Salesperson's Accent as a Globalization Issue, Thunderbird International Business Review, 42 (1), 29-46. Giles, H. and P. F. Powesland (1975), Speech styles and social evaluation. Academic Press. Giles, Howard, Angie Williams, Diane M. Mackie, and Francine Rosselli (1995), Reactions to Anglo- and Hispanic-American-accented speakers: Affect, identity, persuasion, and the English-only controversy, Language and Communication, 15(2), 107-120. Haslam, S. Alexander, Penelope J. Oakes, John C. Turner and Craig McGarty (1995), Social Categorization and Group Homogeneity: Changes in the Perceived Applicability of Stereotype Content as a Function of Comparative Context and Trait Favourableness, British Journal of Social Psychology, 34 (2), 139-160. Hennig-Thurau, Thorsten, Markus Groth, Michael Paul, and Dwayne D Gremler (2006), Are All Smiles Created Equal? How Emotional Contagion and Emotional Labor Affect Service Relationships. Journal of Marketing, 70 (3), 58-73. Hewstone, Miles (1990), The Ultimate Attribution Error?: A Review of the Literature on Intergroup Causal Attribution, European Journal of Social Psychology, 20 (4), 311-335. Hilford, Andy, Murray Glanzer, Kisok Kim, and Lawrence T. DeCarlo (2002), Regularities of Source Recognition: ROC analysis, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 131(4), 494-510. Hosoda, Megumi, Eugene F. Stone-Romero, and Jennifer N. Walter (2007) Listeners Cognitive and Affective Reactions to English Speakers with Standard American English and Asian Sccents, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 104 (1), 307-326. Jackson, Linda, Linda Sullivan and Carole N. Hodge (1993), Stereotype Effects on Attributions, Predictions, and Evaluations: No Two Social Judgments are Quite Alike, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(1), 69-84. Kahneman, Daniel, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky (1982), Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, New York: Cambridge University Press. Ko, Sei Jin, Charles M. Judd, and Irene Blair (2006), What the Voice Reveals: Within- and Between-Category Stereotyping on the Basis of Voice, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(6), 806-819. Kunda, Ziva and Thagard, Paul (1996), Forming Impressions from Stereotypes, Traits, and Behaviors: A Parallel-Constraint-Satisfaction Theory, Psychological Review, 103(2), 284-308. Ladegaard, Hans J. (1998), National Stereotypes and Language Attitudes: The Perception of British, American and Australian language and Culture in Denmark, Language and Communication, 18 (4), 251-274. Levi, Susannah and David B. Pisoni (2007), Indexical and Linguistic Channels in Speech Perception: Some Effects of Voiceovers on Advertising Outcomes, in Psycholinguistic Phenomena in Marketing Communications, ed. Tina M. Lowrey, Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 203-219. Lippi-Green, Rosina L. (1997), English with an Accent: Language, Ideology, and Discrimination in the United States, London and New York: Routledge. Locksley, Anne, Eugene Borgida, Nancy Brekke and Christine Hepburn (1980), Sex Stereotypes and Social Judgment, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 (5), 821-831. Matta, Shashi and Valerie S. Folkes (2005), Inferences about the Brand from Counterstereotypical Service Providers, Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (2), 196-206. Mount, Michael K. and Duane E. Thompson (1987), Cognitive Categorization and Quality of Performance Ratings, Journal of Applied Psychology, 72 (2), 240-247. Mullen, Brian, Brown, Rupert and Smith, Colleen (1992), Ingroup Bias as a function of Salience, Relevance, and Status: An Integration, European Journal of Social Psychology, 22 (2), 1992, 103-122. Oliver, Richard L. (1980), A Cognitive Model of the Antecedents and Consequences of Satisfaction Decisions. Journal of Marketing Research, 17 (4), 460-469. Parasuraman, A., Valarie A. Zeithaml, and Leonard L. Berry (1985), A Conceptual Model of Service Quality and its Implications for Future Research. Journal of Marketing, 49 (4), 41-51. Podberesky, Rosita, Robert H. Deluty and Stanley Feldstein (1990). Evaluations of Spanish- and Oriental-accented English speakers. Social Behavior and Personality, 18 (1), 53-63. Poster, Winifred R. (2007) Who's on the line? Indian call center agents pose as Americans for U.S.-outsourced firms, Industrial Relations, 46 (2), 271-304. Purkiss, Sharon L. Segrest , Pamela L. Perrew, Treena L. Gillespie, Bronston T. Mayes and Gerald R. Ferris (2006), Implicit Sources of Bias in Employment Interview Judgments and Decisions, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 101 (2), 152-167. Sandberg, Jared (2007), 'It Says Press Any Key; Where's the Any Key?': India's Call-Center Workers Get Pounded, Pampered, Wall Street Journal (Eastern edition), Feb 20, B1. Schneider, David J. (2004). The Psychology of Stereotyping, New York: Guilford Press. Seta, J. J, Catherine E. Seta and Todd McElroy (2003), Attributional Biases in the Service of Stereotype Maintenance: A Schema-Maintenance through Compensation Analysis, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29 (2), 151-163. Solomon, Michael R. (1985), A Role Theory Perspective on Dyadic Interactions: The Service Encounter. Journal of Marketing, 49 (1), 99-112. Stockwell, Peter (2002), Sociolinguistics: A Resource Book for Students, Routledge, London and New York. Tsalikis, John, Oscar W. DeShields Jr., and Michael S. LaTour (1991), The Role of Accent on the Credibility and Effectiveness of the Salesperson, The Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 11 (1), 31-42. Yeung, Catherine W. M. and Dilip Soman (2007), The duration heuristic, Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (3), 315-326. UV]^       " N [ e f s x ļıļ˦֘~vodvovjhLWU hLWhLWjhLWU h._hLWh._hLWmH sH hLWh hDh._h._5\h._h mH sH jh._Ujh._U h._h._h._h._mH sH h._hq+ h._hq+ 5\h._hq3X5\h5Qh 5 h 5%TUV^ " N f  B U i $a$gdLW$a$gdD$a$gd._$a$gdq+ $a$gd V< U d h m p q r    H I J U ƿٟ{sk`h 6B*]phh^;|h 5h^;|hD5h^;|hq+ 5 hq+ 5h5Qhq+ 5hDhq3X5 hDh hDh jhDhDUjhDhDU hDhDhDhDmH sH hDmH sH h._hDmH sH hDh 5\hDhD5\hDhq3X#  1 H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U gd1I$a$gdq+ gdq3X$a$gd $a$gdD$a$gd._ VW'HYafz~_`ac"d"e"6$7$:$D$E$ξ᦮ٛጛጛጛጛwh^;|hLW5 h^;|5hLWhP!hXQih B*phhQhXQih1IB*phhQB*phh^;|B*phhM/B*phh1Ihq%hq%B*phhOh1IB*phh B*phh1IB*phh1I6B*]phh>E6B*]ph) ~`ad"e"2$4$5$6$7$8$9$:$gdLWh`hgdQh`hgd^;|h`hgdD$a$gdLWgdD:$E$F$$$5%6%%%X&Y&&&''(())y*z*-+.+ h^h`gdM/gdM/ h^h`gdLWgd^;| h^h`gd^;|gdLW$a$gd^;|E$F$}$$$$$$%"%4%5%%%%%&H&W&X&f&l&v&}&~&&&&&&&''''(((()))))))))))*ƾ h^;|\hLWhM/\hM/hM/6\]hM/hM/\ hM/\hM/hLW6\] hLW\hLWhLW6\hLWhLW\hM/hLWB*phh^;|B*phh^;|h^;|6B*]phh^;|h^;|B*phhLWCJOJQJ^JaJ2**** *!*+*,*2*3*A*f*x*y*z**+,+-+.++,,,,,,,--9-j----$.N._.`.a../////////00111'1z11112;2N2O2P222222e333334D4\4]4^44444hLWh^;|\hLWhM/\ hLW\hLWhLW6\hLWhLW\ h^;|\S.+,,----`.a.////1111O2P22233]4^444gd^;|gdM/ h^h`gdLW445555=6\6l6m6n666 7 7 778888888888999 999: : :q:::::::;;;;;;;;&<B<U<V< hLWhLWhLWhLW6 hLW\hLWhLW6\hLWhLW\hLWhM/\645m6n6 7 788889 9 : :::;;;;U<V<gdLWgdM/ h^h`gdLW h^h`gd^;|21h:pD/ =!"#$% DyK amywang@ku.eduyK Dmailto:amywang@ku.eduyX;H,]ą'cDyK aarndt@odu.eduyK Dmailto:aarndt@odu.eduyX;H,]ą'cDyK ssingh@ku.eduyK Bmailto:ssingh@ku.eduyX;H,]ą'cD@D NormalCJ_HaJmH nHsH tHDA@D Default Paragraph FontRi@R  Table Normal4 l4a (k@(No List6U@6 ._ Hyperlink >*B*phV4 X TUV^"NfBUi 1HIJKLMNOPQRSTU~  `ade2456789:EF56XY !!y"z"-#.#$$%%%%`&a&''''))))O*P***++],^,,,-m.n. / /00001 1 2 2223333U4X4000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000000TUV"NfBUi1HI~  ae2456Y233U4X4Z00HZ00Z00000000Z00~wZ00Z00Z00Z0 0Z0 0Z00Z00Z00U~Z00Z00Z00Z00Z0 0Z0 0Z0 0Z000000J000J000J000H000H000B00 @00 @0@0J000J000J000J000J000J000 @00Z0(0 )Z0(0Z0(0Z0(0)Z0(0Z0(0000 E$*4V<"$'(* :$.+4V< #%&)+V<!qV4XXX8@0(  B S  ?JcRdReR,fR$gRhRiRjR$kRdlR$mRnRoRdpRdqRrRdsR$tRuRvRdwR$xRyRzRd{R$|R}R$~RdRRR$RdRLRLR RRR[R]R\R\R\RRāRRDR4RtR0 RRR4RtRRR4RtRRR4RtRRR4RtRRR4RtRRR4RtR^hrrNNR[yyBBLO  RR{Ogl%v%v%%''((((m)|)))--^0^0111222X4      !"%#$&'()*+,-./0123456789:;<=>?@ABCDEFGHIdp|QZee//JNTTWW~Rjt%%%%''((((z))))--c0c0 111233X4     !"$%#&'()*+,-./0123456789:;<=>?@ABCDEFGHI ;E*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttagsaddress=I*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttags PlaceName=J*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttags PlaceType9G*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttagsplace8B*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttagsCity9A*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttagsState>@*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttags PostalCode:D*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttagsStreetB#*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttagscountry-region>!*urn:schemas-microsoft-com:office:smarttags PersonName WJIJGIEDGBA@IGIJGBA@JIJGIEDBGA@GJIEDGBA@#G!!!!JGIBGAIGJ#GBABG#BAGBG#GABGBAGVXUhip g n #(-4+3AHQYmv &!-!!!z"""""""".#4###% %%%)%0%%%%%a&f&h&l&q&x&'"'(((())))))P*U*W*]*m*s***++,--!----- // /'/4/;/=/C/R/Z/X1e1223 33%3:3@333X4UhiQYY!"##%%''---;.n..[0]03T4X433333333333NefBi:F''--X4X4p"mbPoQ6=Z6%hmO~  l q q+ /7.\#l5NDzkp`<5JMM7F>mPR_Up%C373c QI]SEbri (u hh<1b :Y !7!}X!x#=$l$q%"&u/&zZ'f'^($)eC)E) *>`*.(,K@-;.DJ/0O>0nP0713}12V2"3tE3T3cT3~4],5405Ig5X6x6G7'Z7V08D8'9_:U:;;?; <U;<W<==+&=Le=>:>`>*?1?n?t?y@$@q@A9A?AyAABIRCfD$E2E7E>E FFuF uG.HkUI@JPJ5KHK L L5)LoLM,N}NNYNbNgNOfOvOoPzPLR0R0R=xS53UNIU=U|?V WLWvbWq3XY$Z^(Z^2ZwZ[%[:([L[e[d\G^r^._^O_e_h+`r=`%a:aaaTbca&c$7cvclejf*f:foh h5h>OhxYhYh*khciv_iCjhjel:tmo2HopMp~jpqKqs6sHst,u u5u]vs6vWPw*pwivxSyozzNIzRz2{^{|^;|g|1}*~V~.:){*F. l@[Lsv,+#J hR j4iN Sw&ImJJyzg 9 wY;!3oHxc9?5{HpCK&w2;lqymX<_{S9i& As tDNY^>j'SaS[iy _U>V3^\^g8X1I\e^ 5ft=py&+T<G4UBh;z~. 7~.K(kN\P!C(4YJb4K?TunN.McWyvvV /MMk%@GYM/N6yNy6 >&) Z]ibZhQu#0t@|'5 $.3q1.N|c]|$m{Nf1sxS46t;WL%@P[%(l9a>~[/>^e1&AEo9vx$r-CLwT - bHtN )a|Cq~ 'P* 2X4%O@''D*''V4@UnknownGz Times New Roman5Symbol3& z Arial;[SOSimSun"qh)|Ȧ)|Ȧ{Ȧ,^,^!24d<4<4 2qHX)?4052PThe Impact of Accent Stereotypes on Service Outcomes and Its Boundary ConditionsZWangZWangOh+'0$0D T`    TThe Impact of Accent Stereotypes on Service Outcomes and Its Boundary ConditionsZWang Normal.dotZWang2Microsoft Office Word@@2ur @@,՜.+,D՜.+,L hp  -KU, School of Business^<4' QThe Impact of Accent Stereotypes on Service Outcomes and Its Boundary Conditions Titlel 8@ _PID_HLINKSA$:mailto:ssingh@ku.eduN{mailto:aarndt@odu.edun]mailto:amywang@ku.edu  !"#$%&'()*+,./012346789:;<=>?@ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSUVWXYZ[]^_`abcfRoot Entry FP hData -1Table5-<WordDocument4XSummaryInformation(TDocumentSummaryInformation8\CompObjq  FMicrosoft Office Word Document MSWordDocWord.Document.89q